ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Gero Miesenboeck - Optogeneticist
Using light and a little genetic engineering -- optogenetics -- Gero Miesenboeck has developed a way to control how living nerve cells work, and advanced understanding of how the brain controls behavior.

Why you should listen

Gero Miesenboeck is pioneering the field of optogenetics: genetically modifying nerve cells to respond to light. By flashing light at a modified neuron in a living nervous system, Miesenboeck and his collaborators can mimic a brain impulse -- and then study what happens next. Optogenetics will allow ever more precise experiments on living brains, allowing us to gather better evidence on how electrical impulses on tissue translate into actual behavior and thoughts.

In one experiment, done at Yale, he and his team engineered fruit flies to be light-sensitive in the neural area responsible for escape response. Then the flies were beheaded; fruit flies can live for a day without their heads, but they don't move. When the modified cells were flashed with light, though, the headless flies flew. Miesenboeck had successfully simulated an order from a brain that wasn't even there anymore.

Miesenboeck's current research at Oxford's growing department of neurobiology focuses on the nerve cell networks that underpin what animals perceive, remember and do. In one recent experiment, he used optogenetics to implant an unpleasant memory in a fruit fly, causing it to "remember" to avoid a certain odor as it traveled around. He and his team were able, in fact, to find the fly's specific 12-neuron brain circuit that govern memory formation.

More profile about the speaker
Gero Miesenboeck | Speaker | TED.com
TEDGlobal 2010

Gero Miesenboeck: Re-engineering the brain

Filmed:
717,411 views

In the quest to map the brain, many scientists have attempted the incredibly daunting task of recording the activity of each neuron. Gero Miesenboeck works backward -- manipulating specific neurons to figure out exactly what they do, through a series of stunning experiments that reengineer the way fruit flies percieve light.
- Optogeneticist
Using light and a little genetic engineering -- optogenetics -- Gero Miesenboeck has developed a way to control how living nerve cells work, and advanced understanding of how the brain controls behavior. Full bio

Double-click the English transcript below to play the video.

00:15
I have a doppelganger.
0
0
3000
00:18
(Laughter)
1
3000
3000
00:21
Dr. Gero is a brilliant
2
6000
3000
00:24
but slightly mad scientist
3
9000
2000
00:26
in the "Dragonball Z: Android Saga."
4
11000
3000
00:29
If you look very carefully,
5
14000
2000
00:31
you see that his skull has been replaced
6
16000
3000
00:34
with a transparent Plexiglas dome
7
19000
2000
00:36
so that the workings of his brain can be observed
8
21000
3000
00:39
and also controlled with light.
9
24000
3000
00:42
That's exactly what I do --
10
27000
2000
00:44
optical mind control.
11
29000
2000
00:46
(Laughter)
12
31000
2000
00:48
But in contrast to my evil twin
13
33000
2000
00:50
who lusts after world domination,
14
35000
3000
00:53
my motives are not sinister.
15
38000
3000
00:56
I control the brain
16
41000
2000
00:58
in order to understand how it works.
17
43000
2000
01:00
Now wait a minute, you may say,
18
45000
2000
01:02
how can you go straight to controlling the brain
19
47000
3000
01:05
without understanding it first?
20
50000
2000
01:07
Isn't that putting the cart before the horse?
21
52000
3000
01:11
Many neuroscientists agree with this view
22
56000
3000
01:14
and think that understanding will come
23
59000
3000
01:17
from more detailed observation and analysis.
24
62000
3000
01:20
They say, "If we could record the activity of our neurons,
25
65000
4000
01:24
we would understand the brain."
26
69000
3000
01:27
But think for a moment what that means.
27
72000
3000
01:30
Even if we could measure
28
75000
2000
01:32
what every cell is doing at all times,
29
77000
2000
01:34
we would still have to make sense
30
79000
2000
01:36
of the recorded activity patterns,
31
81000
2000
01:38
and that's so difficult,
32
83000
2000
01:40
chances are we'll understand these patterns
33
85000
2000
01:42
just as little as the brains that produce them.
34
87000
3000
01:45
Take a look at what brain activity might look like.
35
90000
3000
01:48
In this simulation, each black dot
36
93000
2000
01:50
is one nerve cell.
37
95000
2000
01:52
The dot is visible
38
97000
2000
01:54
whenever a cell fires an electrical impulse.
39
99000
2000
01:56
There's 10,000 neurons here.
40
101000
2000
01:58
So you're looking at roughly one percent
41
103000
2000
02:00
of the brain of a cockroach.
42
105000
3000
02:04
Your brains are about 100 million times
43
109000
3000
02:07
more complicated.
44
112000
2000
02:09
Somewhere, in a pattern like this,
45
114000
2000
02:11
is you,
46
116000
2000
02:13
your perceptions,
47
118000
2000
02:15
your emotions, your memories,
48
120000
3000
02:18
your plans for the future.
49
123000
2000
02:20
But we don't know where,
50
125000
2000
02:22
since we don't know how to read the pattern.
51
127000
3000
02:25
We don't understand the code used by the brain.
52
130000
3000
02:28
To make progress,
53
133000
2000
02:30
we need to break the code.
54
135000
2000
02:32
But how?
55
137000
2000
02:35
An experienced code-breaker will tell you
56
140000
2000
02:37
that in order to figure out what the symbols in a code mean,
57
142000
3000
02:40
it's essential to be able to play with them,
58
145000
3000
02:43
to rearrange them at will.
59
148000
2000
02:45
So in this situation too,
60
150000
2000
02:47
to decode the information
61
152000
2000
02:49
contained in patterns like this,
62
154000
2000
02:51
watching alone won't do.
63
156000
2000
02:53
We need to rearrange the pattern.
64
158000
2000
02:55
In other words,
65
160000
2000
02:57
instead of recording the activity of neurons,
66
162000
2000
02:59
we need to control it.
67
164000
2000
03:01
It's not essential that we can control
68
166000
2000
03:03
the activity of all neurons in the brain, just some.
69
168000
3000
03:06
The more targeted our interventions, the better.
70
171000
2000
03:08
And I'll show you in a moment
71
173000
2000
03:10
how we can achieve the necessary precision.
72
175000
3000
03:13
And since I'm realistic, rather than grandiose,
73
178000
3000
03:16
I don't claim that the ability to control the function of the nervous system
74
181000
3000
03:19
will at once unravel all its mysteries.
75
184000
3000
03:22
But we'll certainly learn a lot.
76
187000
3000
03:27
Now, I'm by no means
77
192000
2000
03:29
the first person to realize
78
194000
2000
03:31
how powerful a tool intervention is.
79
196000
3000
03:34
The history of attempts
80
199000
2000
03:36
to tinker with the function of the nervous system
81
201000
2000
03:38
is long and illustrious.
82
203000
2000
03:40
It dates back at least 200 years,
83
205000
3000
03:43
to Galvani's famous experiments
84
208000
2000
03:45
in the late 18th century and beyond.
85
210000
3000
03:49
Galvani showed that a frog's legs twitched
86
214000
3000
03:52
when he connected the lumbar nerve
87
217000
2000
03:54
to a source of electrical current.
88
219000
3000
03:57
This experiment revealed the first, and perhaps most fundamental,
89
222000
3000
04:00
nugget of the neural code:
90
225000
2000
04:02
that information is written in the form
91
227000
2000
04:04
of electrical impulses.
92
229000
3000
04:08
Galvani's approach
93
233000
2000
04:10
of probing the nervous system with electrodes
94
235000
2000
04:12
has remained state-of-the-art until today,
95
237000
3000
04:15
despite a number of drawbacks.
96
240000
3000
04:18
Sticking wires into the brain is obviously rather crude.
97
243000
3000
04:21
It's hard to do in animals that run around,
98
246000
2000
04:23
and there is a physical limit
99
248000
2000
04:25
to the number of wires
100
250000
2000
04:27
that can be inserted simultaneously.
101
252000
3000
04:30
So around the turn of the last century,
102
255000
2000
04:32
I started to think,
103
257000
2000
04:34
"Wouldn't it be wonderful if one could take this logic
104
259000
3000
04:37
and turn it upside down?"
105
262000
2000
04:39
So instead of inserting a wire
106
264000
2000
04:41
into one spot of the brain,
107
266000
3000
04:44
re-engineer the brain itself
108
269000
2000
04:46
so that some of its neural elements
109
271000
3000
04:49
become responsive to diffusely broadcast signals
110
274000
3000
04:52
such as a flash of light.
111
277000
3000
04:55
Such an approach would literally, in a flash of light,
112
280000
3000
04:58
overcome many of the obstacles to discovery.
113
283000
3000
05:01
First, it's clearly a non-invasive,
114
286000
3000
05:04
wireless form of communication.
115
289000
3000
05:07
And second, just as in a radio broadcast,
116
292000
2000
05:09
you can communicate with many receivers at once.
117
294000
3000
05:12
You don't need to know where these receivers are,
118
297000
3000
05:15
and it doesn't matter if these receivers move --
119
300000
2000
05:17
just think of the stereo in your car.
120
302000
3000
05:20
It gets even better,
121
305000
3000
05:23
for it turns out that we can fabricate the receivers
122
308000
3000
05:26
out of materials that are encoded in DNA.
123
311000
3000
05:29
So each nerve cell
124
314000
2000
05:31
with the right genetic makeup
125
316000
2000
05:33
will spontaneously produce a receiver
126
318000
3000
05:36
that allows us to control its function.
127
321000
3000
05:39
I hope you'll appreciate
128
324000
2000
05:41
the beautiful simplicity
129
326000
2000
05:43
of this concept.
130
328000
2000
05:45
There's no high-tech gizmos here,
131
330000
2000
05:47
just biology revealed through biology.
132
332000
3000
05:51
Now let's take a look at these miraculous receivers up close.
133
336000
3000
05:54
As we zoom in on one of these purple neurons,
134
339000
3000
05:57
we see that its outer membrane
135
342000
2000
05:59
is studded with microscopic pores.
136
344000
2000
06:01
Pores like these conduct electrical current
137
346000
2000
06:03
and are responsible
138
348000
2000
06:05
for all the communication in the nervous system.
139
350000
2000
06:07
But these pores here are special.
140
352000
2000
06:09
They are coupled to light receptors
141
354000
2000
06:11
similar to the ones in your eyes.
142
356000
3000
06:14
Whenever a flash of light hits the receptor,
143
359000
2000
06:16
the pore opens, an electrical current is switched on,
144
361000
3000
06:19
and the neuron fires electrical impulses.
145
364000
3000
06:22
Because the light-activated pore is encoded in DNA,
146
367000
3000
06:25
we can achieve incredible precision.
147
370000
3000
06:28
This is because,
148
373000
2000
06:30
although each cell in our bodies
149
375000
2000
06:32
contains the same set of genes,
150
377000
2000
06:34
different mixes of genes get turned on and off
151
379000
2000
06:36
in different cells.
152
381000
2000
06:38
You can exploit this to make sure
153
383000
2000
06:40
that only some neurons
154
385000
2000
06:42
contain our light-activated pore and others don't.
155
387000
3000
06:45
So in this cartoon, the bluish white cell
156
390000
2000
06:47
in the upper-left corner
157
392000
2000
06:49
does not respond to light
158
394000
2000
06:51
because it lacks the light-activated pore.
159
396000
3000
06:54
The approach works so well
160
399000
2000
06:56
that we can write purely artificial messages
161
401000
2000
06:58
directly to the brain.
162
403000
2000
07:00
In this example, each electrical impulse,
163
405000
2000
07:02
each deflection on the trace,
164
407000
3000
07:05
is caused by a brief pulse of light.
165
410000
3000
07:08
And the approach, of course, also works
166
413000
2000
07:10
in moving, behaving animals.
167
415000
3000
07:13
This is the first ever such experiment,
168
418000
2000
07:15
sort of the optical equivalent of Galvani's.
169
420000
3000
07:18
It was done six or seven years ago
170
423000
2000
07:20
by my then graduate student, Susana Lima.
171
425000
3000
07:23
Susana had engineered the fruit fly on the left
172
428000
3000
07:26
so that just two out of the 200,000 cells in its brain
173
431000
4000
07:30
expressed the light-activated pore.
174
435000
3000
07:33
You're familiar with these cells
175
438000
2000
07:35
because they are the ones that frustrate you
176
440000
2000
07:37
when you try to swat the fly.
177
442000
2000
07:39
They trained the escape reflex that makes the fly jump into the air
178
444000
3000
07:42
and fly away whenever you move your hand in position.
179
447000
3000
07:46
And you can see here that the flash of light has exactly the same effect.
180
451000
3000
07:49
The animal jumps, it spreads its wings, it vibrates them,
181
454000
3000
07:52
but it can't actually take off
182
457000
2000
07:54
because the fly is sandwiched between two glass plates.
183
459000
3000
07:58
Now to make sure that this was no reaction of the fly
184
463000
2000
08:00
to a flash it could see,
185
465000
3000
08:03
Susana did a simple
186
468000
2000
08:05
but brutally effective experiment.
187
470000
2000
08:07
She cut the heads off of her flies.
188
472000
3000
08:11
These headless bodies can live for about a day,
189
476000
3000
08:14
but they don't do much.
190
479000
2000
08:16
They just stand around
191
481000
3000
08:19
and groom excessively.
192
484000
3000
08:22
So it seems that the only trait that survives decapitation is vanity.
193
487000
3000
08:25
(Laughter)
194
490000
3000
08:30
Anyway, as you'll see in a moment,
195
495000
2000
08:32
Susana was able to turn on the flight motor
196
497000
3000
08:35
of what's the equivalent of the spinal cord of these flies
197
500000
3000
08:38
and get some of the headless bodies
198
503000
2000
08:40
to actually take off and fly away.
199
505000
3000
08:47
They didn't get very far, obviously.
200
512000
2000
08:50
Since we took these first steps,
201
515000
2000
08:52
the field of optogenetics has exploded.
202
517000
3000
08:55
And there are now hundreds of labs
203
520000
2000
08:57
using these approaches.
204
522000
2000
08:59
And we've come a long way
205
524000
2000
09:01
since Galvani's and Susana's first successes
206
526000
3000
09:04
in making animals twitch or jump.
207
529000
2000
09:06
We can now actually interfere with their psychology
208
531000
3000
09:09
in rather profound ways,
209
534000
2000
09:11
as I'll show you in my last example,
210
536000
2000
09:13
which is directed at a familiar question.
211
538000
3000
09:16
Life is a string of choices
212
541000
3000
09:19
creating a constant pressure to decide what to do next.
213
544000
3000
09:23
We cope with this pressure by having brains,
214
548000
3000
09:26
and within our brains, decision-making centers
215
551000
3000
09:29
that I've called here the "Actor."
216
554000
3000
09:33
The Actor implements a policy that takes into account
217
558000
3000
09:36
the state of the environment
218
561000
2000
09:38
and the context in which we operate.
219
563000
3000
09:41
Our actions change the environment, or context,
220
566000
3000
09:44
and these changes are then fed back into the decision loop.
221
569000
3000
09:48
Now to put some neurobiological meat
222
573000
3000
09:51
on this abstract model,
223
576000
2000
09:53
we constructed a simple one-dimensional world
224
578000
2000
09:55
for our favorite subject, fruit flies.
225
580000
3000
09:58
Each chamber in these two vertical stacks
226
583000
2000
10:00
contains one fly.
227
585000
2000
10:02
The left and the right halves of the chamber
228
587000
3000
10:05
are filled with two different odors,
229
590000
2000
10:07
and a security camera watches
230
592000
2000
10:09
as the flies pace up and down between them.
231
594000
3000
10:12
Here's some such CCTV footage.
232
597000
2000
10:14
Whenever a fly reaches the midpoint of the chamber
233
599000
3000
10:17
where the two odor streams meet,
234
602000
2000
10:19
it has to make a decision.
235
604000
2000
10:21
It has to decide whether to turn around
236
606000
2000
10:23
and stay in the same odor,
237
608000
2000
10:25
or whether to cross the midline
238
610000
2000
10:27
and try something new.
239
612000
2000
10:29
These decisions are clearly a reflection
240
614000
3000
10:32
of the Actor's policy.
241
617000
3000
10:36
Now for an intelligent being like our fly,
242
621000
3000
10:39
this policy is not written in stone
243
624000
3000
10:42
but rather changes as the animal learns from experience.
244
627000
3000
10:45
We can incorporate such an element
245
630000
2000
10:47
of adaptive intelligence into our model
246
632000
3000
10:50
by assuming that the fly's brain
247
635000
2000
10:52
contains not only an Actor,
248
637000
2000
10:54
but a different group of cells,
249
639000
2000
10:56
a "Critic," that provides a running commentary
250
641000
3000
10:59
on the Actor's choices.
251
644000
2000
11:01
You can think of this nagging inner voice
252
646000
3000
11:04
as sort of the brain's equivalent
253
649000
2000
11:06
of the Catholic Church,
254
651000
2000
11:08
if you're an Austrian like me,
255
653000
3000
11:11
or the super-ego, if you're Freudian,
256
656000
3000
11:14
or your mother, if you're Jewish.
257
659000
2000
11:16
(Laughter)
258
661000
4000
11:20
Now obviously,
259
665000
2000
11:22
the Critic is a key ingredient
260
667000
3000
11:25
in what makes us intelligent.
261
670000
2000
11:27
So we set out to identify
262
672000
2000
11:29
the cells in the fly's brain
263
674000
2000
11:31
that played the role of the Critic.
264
676000
2000
11:33
And the logic of our experiment was simple.
265
678000
3000
11:36
We thought if we could use our optical remote control
266
681000
3000
11:39
to activate the cells of the Critic,
267
684000
3000
11:42
we should be able, artificially, to nag the Actor
268
687000
3000
11:45
into changing its policy.
269
690000
2000
11:47
In other words,
270
692000
2000
11:49
the fly should learn from mistakes
271
694000
2000
11:51
that it thought it had made
272
696000
2000
11:53
but, in reality, it had not made.
273
698000
3000
11:56
So we bred flies
274
701000
2000
11:58
whose brains were more or less randomly peppered
275
703000
3000
12:01
with cells that were light addressable.
276
706000
2000
12:03
And then we took these flies
277
708000
2000
12:05
and allowed them to make choices.
278
710000
2000
12:07
And whenever they made one of the two choices,
279
712000
2000
12:09
chose one odor,
280
714000
2000
12:11
in this case the blue one over the orange one,
281
716000
2000
12:13
we switched on the lights.
282
718000
2000
12:15
If the Critic was among the optically activated cells,
283
720000
3000
12:18
the result of this intervention
284
723000
2000
12:20
should be a change in policy.
285
725000
3000
12:23
The fly should learn to avoid
286
728000
2000
12:25
the optically reinforced odor.
287
730000
2000
12:27
Here's what happened in two instances:
288
732000
3000
12:30
We're comparing two strains of flies,
289
735000
3000
12:33
each of them having
290
738000
2000
12:35
about 100 light-addressable cells in their brains,
291
740000
2000
12:37
shown here in green on the left and on the right.
292
742000
3000
12:40
What's common among these groups of cells
293
745000
3000
12:43
is that they all produce the neurotransmitter dopamine.
294
748000
3000
12:46
But the identities of the individual
295
751000
2000
12:48
dopamine-producing neurons
296
753000
2000
12:50
are clearly largely different on the left and on the right.
297
755000
3000
12:53
Optically activating
298
758000
2000
12:55
these hundred or so cells
299
760000
2000
12:57
into two strains of flies
300
762000
2000
12:59
has dramatically different consequences.
301
764000
2000
13:01
If you look first at the behavior
302
766000
2000
13:03
of the fly on the right,
303
768000
2000
13:05
you can see that whenever it reaches the midpoint of the chamber
304
770000
3000
13:08
where the two odors meet,
305
773000
2000
13:10
it marches straight through, as it did before.
306
775000
3000
13:13
Its behavior is completely unchanged.
307
778000
2000
13:15
But the behavior of the fly on the left is very different.
308
780000
3000
13:18
Whenever it comes up to the midpoint,
309
783000
3000
13:21
it pauses,
310
786000
2000
13:23
it carefully scans the odor interface
311
788000
2000
13:25
as if it was sniffing out its environment,
312
790000
2000
13:27
and then it turns around.
313
792000
2000
13:29
This means that the policy that the Actor implements
314
794000
3000
13:32
now includes an instruction to avoid the odor
315
797000
2000
13:34
that's in the right half of the chamber.
316
799000
3000
13:37
This means that the Critic
317
802000
2000
13:39
must have spoken in that animal,
318
804000
2000
13:41
and that the Critic must be contained
319
806000
2000
13:43
among the dopamine-producing neurons on the left,
320
808000
3000
13:46
but not among the dopamine producing neurons on the right.
321
811000
3000
13:49
Through many such experiments,
322
814000
3000
13:52
we were able to narrow down
323
817000
2000
13:54
the identity of the Critic
324
819000
2000
13:56
to just 12 cells.
325
821000
2000
13:58
These 12 cells, as shown here in green,
326
823000
3000
14:01
send the output to a brain structure
327
826000
2000
14:03
called the "mushroom body,"
328
828000
2000
14:05
which is shown here in gray.
329
830000
2000
14:07
We know from our formal model
330
832000
2000
14:09
that the brain structure
331
834000
2000
14:11
at the receiving end of the Critic's commentary is the Actor.
332
836000
3000
14:14
So this anatomy suggests
333
839000
2000
14:16
that the mushroom bodies have something to do
334
841000
3000
14:19
with action choice.
335
844000
2000
14:21
Based on everything we know about the mushroom bodies,
336
846000
2000
14:23
this makes perfect sense.
337
848000
2000
14:25
In fact, it makes so much sense
338
850000
2000
14:27
that we can construct an electronic toy circuit
339
852000
3000
14:30
that simulates the behavior of the fly.
340
855000
3000
14:33
In this electronic toy circuit,
341
858000
3000
14:36
the mushroom body neurons are symbolized
342
861000
2000
14:38
by the vertical bank of blue LEDs
343
863000
3000
14:41
in the center of the board.
344
866000
3000
14:44
These LED's are wired to sensors
345
869000
2000
14:46
that detect the presence of odorous molecules in the air.
346
871000
3000
14:50
Each odor activates a different combination of sensors,
347
875000
3000
14:53
which in turn activates
348
878000
2000
14:55
a different odor detector in the mushroom body.
349
880000
3000
14:58
So the pilot in the cockpit of the fly,
350
883000
2000
15:00
the Actor,
351
885000
2000
15:02
can tell which odor is present
352
887000
2000
15:04
simply by looking at which of the blue LEDs lights up.
353
889000
4000
15:09
What the Actor does with this information
354
894000
2000
15:11
depends on its policy,
355
896000
2000
15:13
which is stored in the strengths of the connection,
356
898000
2000
15:15
between the odor detectors
357
900000
2000
15:17
and the motors
358
902000
2000
15:19
that power the fly's evasive actions.
359
904000
2000
15:22
If the connection is weak, the motors will stay off
360
907000
2000
15:24
and the fly will continue straight on its course.
361
909000
3000
15:27
If the connection is strong, the motors will turn on
362
912000
3000
15:30
and the fly will initiate a turn.
363
915000
3000
15:33
Now consider a situation
364
918000
2000
15:35
in which the motors stay off,
365
920000
2000
15:37
the fly continues on its path
366
922000
3000
15:40
and it suffers some painful consequence
367
925000
2000
15:42
such as getting zapped.
368
927000
2000
15:44
In a situation like this,
369
929000
2000
15:46
we would expect the Critic to speak up
370
931000
2000
15:48
and to tell the Actor
371
933000
2000
15:50
to change its policy.
372
935000
2000
15:52
We have created such a situation, artificially,
373
937000
3000
15:55
by turning on the critic with a flash of light.
374
940000
3000
15:58
That caused a strengthening of the connections
375
943000
3000
16:01
between the currently active odor detector
376
946000
3000
16:04
and the motors.
377
949000
2000
16:06
So the next time
378
951000
2000
16:08
the fly finds itself facing the same odor again,
379
953000
3000
16:11
the connection is strong enough to turn on the motors
380
956000
3000
16:14
and to trigger an evasive maneuver.
381
959000
3000
16:19
I don't know about you,
382
964000
3000
16:22
but I find it exhilarating to see
383
967000
3000
16:25
how vague psychological notions
384
970000
3000
16:28
evaporate and give rise
385
973000
2000
16:30
to a physical, mechanistic understanding of the mind,
386
975000
3000
16:33
even if it's the mind of the fly.
387
978000
3000
16:36
This is one piece of good news.
388
981000
3000
16:39
The other piece of good news,
389
984000
2000
16:41
for a scientist at least,
390
986000
2000
16:43
is that much remains to be discovered.
391
988000
3000
16:46
In the experiments I told you about,
392
991000
2000
16:48
we have lifted the identity of the Critic,
393
993000
3000
16:51
but we still have no idea
394
996000
2000
16:53
how the Critic does its job.
395
998000
2000
16:55
Come to think of it, knowing when you're wrong
396
1000000
2000
16:57
without a teacher, or your mother, telling you,
397
1002000
3000
17:00
is a very hard problem.
398
1005000
2000
17:02
There are some ideas in computer science
399
1007000
2000
17:04
and in artificial intelligence
400
1009000
2000
17:06
as to how this might be done,
401
1011000
2000
17:08
but we still haven't solved
402
1013000
2000
17:10
a single example
403
1015000
2000
17:12
of how intelligent behavior
404
1017000
3000
17:15
springs from the physical interactions
405
1020000
2000
17:17
in living matter.
406
1022000
2000
17:19
I think we'll get there in the not too distant future.
407
1024000
3000
17:22
Thank you.
408
1027000
2000
17:24
(Applause)
409
1029000
4000

▲Back to top

ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Gero Miesenboeck - Optogeneticist
Using light and a little genetic engineering -- optogenetics -- Gero Miesenboeck has developed a way to control how living nerve cells work, and advanced understanding of how the brain controls behavior.

Why you should listen

Gero Miesenboeck is pioneering the field of optogenetics: genetically modifying nerve cells to respond to light. By flashing light at a modified neuron in a living nervous system, Miesenboeck and his collaborators can mimic a brain impulse -- and then study what happens next. Optogenetics will allow ever more precise experiments on living brains, allowing us to gather better evidence on how electrical impulses on tissue translate into actual behavior and thoughts.

In one experiment, done at Yale, he and his team engineered fruit flies to be light-sensitive in the neural area responsible for escape response. Then the flies were beheaded; fruit flies can live for a day without their heads, but they don't move. When the modified cells were flashed with light, though, the headless flies flew. Miesenboeck had successfully simulated an order from a brain that wasn't even there anymore.

Miesenboeck's current research at Oxford's growing department of neurobiology focuses on the nerve cell networks that underpin what animals perceive, remember and do. In one recent experiment, he used optogenetics to implant an unpleasant memory in a fruit fly, causing it to "remember" to avoid a certain odor as it traveled around. He and his team were able, in fact, to find the fly's specific 12-neuron brain circuit that govern memory formation.

More profile about the speaker
Gero Miesenboeck | Speaker | TED.com

Data provided by TED.

This site was created in May 2015 and the last update was on January 12, 2020. It will no longer be updated.

We are currently creating a new site called "eng.lish.video" and would be grateful if you could access it.

If you have any questions or suggestions, please feel free to write comments in your language on the contact form.

Privacy Policy

Developer's Blog

Buy Me A Coffee