ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Greg Gage - Neuroscientist
TED Fellow Greg Gage helps kids investigate the neuroscience in their own backyards.

Why you should listen

As half of Backyard Brains, neuroscientist and engineer Greg Gage builds the SpikerBox -- a small rig that helps kids understand the electrical impulses that control the nervous system. He's passionate about helping students understand (viscerally) how our brains and our neurons work, because, as he said onstage at TED2012, we still know very little about how the brain works -- and we need to start inspiring kids early to want to know more.

Before becoming a neuroscientist, Gage worked as an electrical engineer making touchscreens. As he told the Huffington Post: "Scientific equipment in general is pretty expensive, but it's silly because before [getting my PhD in neuroscience] I was an electrical engineer, and you could see that you could make it yourself. So we started as a way to have fun, to show off to our colleagues, but we were also going into classrooms around that time and we thought, wouldn't it be cool if you could bring these gadgets with us so the stuff we were doing in advanced Ph.D. programs in neuroscience, you could also do in fifth grade?" His latest pieces of gear: the Roboroach, a cockroach fitted with an electric backpack that makes it turn on command, and BYB SmartScope, a smartphone-powered microscope.

More profile about the speaker
Greg Gage | Speaker | TED.com
TED2017

Greg Gage: Electrical experiments with plants that count and communicate

Filmed:
3,342,326 views

Neuroscientist Greg Gage takes sophisticated equipment used to study the brain out of graduate-level labs and brings them to middle- and high-school classrooms (and, sometimes, to the TED stage.) Prepare to be amazed as he hooks up the Mimosa pudica, a plant whose leaves close when touched, and the Venus flytrap to an EKG to show us how plants use electrical signals to convey information, prompt movement and even count.
- Neuroscientist
TED Fellow Greg Gage helps kids investigate the neuroscience in their own backyards. Full bio

Double-click the English transcript below to play the video.

00:12
I'm a neuroscientist,
0
760
1256
00:14
and I'm the co-founder of Backyard Brains,
1
2040
2296
00:16
and our mission is to train
the next generation of neuroscientists
2
4360
3856
00:20
by taking graduate-level
neuroscience research equipment
3
8240
3016
00:23
and making it available for kids
in middle schools and high schools.
4
11280
3440
00:27
And so when we go into the classroom,
5
15520
1856
00:29
one way to get them thinking
about the brain, which is very complex,
6
17400
3896
00:33
is to ask them a very simple
question about neuroscience,
7
21320
2936
00:36
and that is, "What has a brain?"
8
24280
2040
00:39
When we ask that,
9
27040
1256
00:40
students will instantly tell you
that their cat or dog has a brain,
10
28320
4055
00:44
and most will say that a mouse
or even a small insect has a brain,
11
32400
4576
00:49
but almost nobody says
that a plant or a tree
12
37000
2816
00:51
or a shrub has a brain.
13
39840
2176
00:54
And so when you push --
14
42040
2496
00:56
because this could actually
help describe a little bit
15
44560
2576
00:59
how the brain actually functions --
16
47160
2176
01:01
so you push and say,
17
49360
1216
01:02
"Well, what is it that makes
living things have brains versus not?"
18
50600
3536
01:06
And often they'll come back
with the classification
19
54160
2456
01:08
that things that move tend to have brains.
20
56640
3776
01:12
And that's absolutely correct.
21
60440
1616
01:14
Our nervous system evolved
because it is electrical.
22
62080
2456
01:16
It's fast, so we can quickly respond
to stimuli in the world
23
64560
3136
01:19
and move if we need to.
24
67720
2216
01:21
But you can go back
and push back on a student,
25
69960
2216
01:24
and say, "Well, you know,
you say that plants don't have brains,
26
72200
3056
01:27
but plants do move."
27
75280
1456
01:28
Anyone who has grown a plant
28
76760
1856
01:30
has noticed that the plant will move
29
78640
1976
01:32
and face the sun.
30
80640
1816
01:34
But they'll say,
"But that's a slow movement.
31
82480
2136
01:36
You know, that doesn't count.
That could be a chemical process."
32
84640
3016
01:39
But what about fast-moving plants?
33
87680
2376
01:42
Now, in 1760, Arthur Dobbs,
the Royal Governor of North Carolina,
34
90080
5096
01:47
made a pretty fascinating discovery.
35
95200
2536
01:49
In the swamps behind his house,
36
97760
2856
01:52
he found a plant that would spring shut
37
100640
3656
01:56
every time a bug would fall in between it.
38
104320
2680
01:59
He called this plant the flytrap,
39
107720
2936
02:02
and within a decade,
it made its way over to Europe,
40
110680
2936
02:05
where eventually the great Charles Darwin
got to study this plant,
41
113640
3536
02:09
and this plant absolutely blew him away.
42
117200
2016
02:11
He called it the most wonderful
plant in the world.
43
119240
2536
02:13
This is a plant
that was an evolutionary wonder.
44
121800
2256
02:16
This is a plant that moves quickly,
45
124080
1976
02:18
which is rare,
46
126080
1296
02:19
and it's carnivorous, which is also rare.
47
127400
1976
02:21
And this is in the same plant.
48
129400
1456
02:22
But I'm here today to tell you
49
130880
1456
02:24
that's not even the coolest thing
about this plant.
50
132360
2416
02:26
The coolest thing
is that the plant can count.
51
134800
2520
02:30
So in order to show that,
52
138560
1376
02:31
we have to get some vocabulary
out of the way.
53
139960
2176
02:34
So I'm going to do what we do
in the classroom with students.
54
142160
3256
02:37
We're going to do
an experiment on electrophysiology,
55
145440
3856
02:41
which is the recording
of the body's electrical signal,
56
149320
3136
02:44
either coming from neurons
or from muscles.
57
152480
2296
02:46
And I'm putting some electrodes
here on my wrists.
58
154800
2336
02:49
As I hook them up,
59
157160
1336
02:50
we're going to be able to see a signal
60
158520
2176
02:52
on the screen here.
61
160720
1456
02:54
And this signal may be familiar to you.
62
162200
1896
02:56
It's called the EKG,
or the electrocardiogram.
63
164120
2176
02:58
And this is coming
from neurons in my heart
64
166320
2456
03:00
that are firing
what's called action potentials,
65
168800
2536
03:03
potential meaning voltage and action
meaning it moves quickly up and down,
66
171360
3856
03:07
which causes my heart to fire,
67
175240
1456
03:08
which then causes
the signal that you see here.
68
176720
2816
03:11
And so I want you to remember the shape
of what we'll be looking at right here,
69
179560
3736
03:15
because this is going to be important.
70
183320
1856
03:17
This is a way that the brain
encodes information
71
185200
2416
03:19
in the form of an action potential.
72
187640
1696
03:21
So now let's turn to some plants.
73
189360
2320
03:24
So I'm going to first
introduce you to the mimosa,
74
192920
3536
03:28
not the drink, but the Mimosa pudica,
75
196480
3216
03:31
and this is a plant that's found
in Central America and South America,
76
199720
3336
03:35
and it has behaviors.
77
203080
1976
03:37
And the first behavior
I'm going to show you
78
205080
2096
03:39
is if I touch the leaves here,
79
207200
2096
03:41
you get to see that the leaves
tend to curl up.
80
209320
2200
03:45
And then the second behavior is,
81
213280
2256
03:47
if I tap the leaf,
82
215560
2176
03:49
the entire branch seems to fall down.
83
217760
1816
03:51
So why does it do that?
84
219600
1536
03:53
It's not really known to science.
85
221160
1616
03:54
One of the reasons why
could be that it scares away insects
86
222800
3216
03:58
or it looks less appealing to herbivores.
87
226040
2176
04:00
But how does it do that?
Now, that's interesting.
88
228240
2496
04:02
We can do an experiment to find out.
89
230760
2136
04:04
So what we're going to do now,
90
232920
1456
04:06
just like I recorded
the electrical potential from my body,
91
234400
3416
04:09
we're going to record the electrical
potential from this plant, this mimosa.
92
237840
3816
04:13
And so what we're going to do
is I've got a wire wrapped around the stem,
93
241680
5496
04:19
and I've got the ground electrode where?
94
247200
2080
04:22
In the ground. It's an electrical
engineering joke. Alright.
95
250360
2936
04:25
(Laughter)
96
253320
1336
04:26
Alright. So I'm going to go ahead
and tap the leaf here,
97
254680
2736
04:29
and I want you to look
at the electrical recording
98
257440
2375
04:31
that we're going to see inside the plant.
99
259839
1961
04:34
Whoa. It is so big,
I've got to scale it down.
100
262520
2896
04:37
Alright. So what is that?
101
265440
1416
04:38
That is an action potential
that is happening inside the plant.
102
266880
2976
04:41
Why was it happening?
103
269880
1256
04:43
Because it wanted to move. Right?
104
271160
1616
04:44
And so when I hit the touch receptors,
105
272800
3016
04:47
it sent a voltage all the way down
to the end of the stem,
106
275840
3176
04:51
which caused it to move.
107
279040
1256
04:52
And now, in our arms,
we would move our muscles,
108
280320
2256
04:54
but the plant doesn't have muscles.
109
282600
1696
04:56
What it has is water inside the cells
110
284320
2256
04:58
and when the voltage hits it,
it opens up, releases the water,
111
286600
2936
05:01
changes the shape of the cells,
and the leaf falls.
112
289560
2434
05:04
OK. So here we see an action potential
encoding information to move. Alright?
113
292480
4936
05:09
But can it do more?
114
297440
1496
05:10
So let's go to find out.
115
298960
1256
05:12
We're going to go to our good friend,
the Venus flytrap here,
116
300240
3056
05:15
and we're going to take a look
at what happens inside the leaf
117
303320
4416
05:19
when a fly lands on here.
118
307760
1936
05:21
So I'm going to pretend
to be a fly right now.
119
309720
2696
05:24
And now here's my Venus flytrap,
120
312440
1656
05:26
and inside the leaf,
you're going to notice
121
314120
2016
05:28
that there are three little hairs here,
and those are trigger hairs.
122
316160
3216
05:31
And so when a fly lands --
123
319400
1376
05:32
I'm going to touch
one of the hairs right now.
124
320800
2496
05:35
Ready? One, two, three.
125
323320
1440
05:39
What do we get? We get
a beautiful action potential.
126
327000
2456
05:41
However, the flytrap doesn't close.
127
329480
2520
05:44
And to understand why that is,
128
332640
1456
05:46
we need to know a little bit more
about the behavior of the flytrap.
129
334120
3216
05:49
Number one is that it takes
a long time to open the traps back up --
130
337360
3296
05:52
you know, about 24 to 48 hours
if there's no fly inside of it.
131
340680
4176
05:56
And so it takes a lot of energy.
132
344880
1696
05:58
And two, it doesn't need to eat
that many flies throughout the year.
133
346600
3216
06:01
Only a handful. It gets
most of its energy from the sun.
134
349840
2656
06:04
It's just trying to replace
some nutrients in the ground with flies.
135
352520
3216
06:07
And the third thing is,
136
355760
1256
06:09
it only opens then closes the traps
a handful of times
137
357040
2976
06:12
until that trap dies.
138
360040
1640
06:14
So therefore, it wants
to make really darn sure
139
362120
2696
06:16
that there's a meal inside of it
before the flytrap snaps shut.
140
364840
4416
06:21
So how does it do that?
141
369280
1200
06:23
It counts the number of seconds
142
371280
2616
06:25
between successive
touching of those hairs.
143
373920
3216
06:29
And so the idea is
that there's a high probability,
144
377160
2416
06:31
if there's a fly inside of there,
that it's going to be clicked together,
145
379600
3456
06:35
and so when it gets the first
action potential,
146
383080
2216
06:37
it starts counting, one, two,
147
385320
1416
06:38
and if it gets to 20
and it doesn't fire again,
148
386760
2216
06:41
then it's not going to close,
149
389000
1416
06:42
but if it does it within there,
then the flytrap will close.
150
390440
2856
06:45
So we're going to go back now.
151
393320
1456
06:46
I'm going to touch
the Venus flytrap again.
152
394800
2016
06:48
I've been talking
for more than 20 seconds.
153
396840
2016
06:50
So we can see what happens
when I touch the hair a second time.
154
398880
2960
06:55
So what do we get?
We get a second action potential,
155
403720
2456
06:58
but again, the leaf doesn't close.
156
406200
1896
07:00
So now if I go back in there
157
408120
1936
07:02
and if I'm a fly moving around,
158
410080
1896
07:04
I'm going to be touching
the leaf a few times.
159
412000
2176
07:06
I'm going to go and brush it a few times.
160
414200
2416
07:08
And immediately,
161
416640
1416
07:10
the flytrap closes.
162
418080
1736
07:11
So here we are seeing the flytrap
actually doing a computation.
163
419840
4136
07:16
It's determining
if there's a fly inside the trap,
164
424000
2496
07:18
and then it closes.
165
426520
1496
07:20
So let's go back to our original question.
166
428040
2400
07:23
Do plants have brains?
167
431520
2296
07:25
Well, the answer is no.
168
433840
1576
07:27
There's no brains in here.
169
435440
1376
07:28
There's no axons, no neurons.
170
436840
3576
07:32
It doesn't get depressed.
171
440440
1376
07:33
It doesn't want to know
what the Tigers' score is.
172
441840
2376
07:36
It doesn't have
self-actualization problems.
173
444240
2096
07:38
But what it does have
is something that's very similar to us,
174
446360
3416
07:41
which is the ability
to communicate using electricity.
175
449800
3056
07:44
It just uses slightly
different ions than we do,
176
452880
2256
07:47
but it's actually doing the same thing.
177
455160
2016
07:49
So just to show you
178
457200
2176
07:51
the ubiquitous nature
of these action potentials,
179
459400
3296
07:54
we saw it in the Venus flytrap,
180
462720
2216
07:56
we've seen an action
potential in the mimosa.
181
464960
2136
07:59
We've even seen
an action potential in a human.
182
467120
2376
08:01
Now, this is the euro of the brain.
183
469520
3216
08:04
It's the way that all
information is passed.
184
472760
2136
08:06
And so what we can do
is we can use those action potentials
185
474920
2816
08:09
to pass information
186
477760
1496
08:11
between species of plants.
187
479280
1736
08:13
And so this is our interspecies
plant-to-plant communicator,
188
481040
4416
08:17
and what we've done
is we've created a brand new experiment
189
485480
3176
08:20
where we're going to record
the action potential from a Venus flytrap,
190
488680
3536
08:24
and we're going to send it
into the sensitive mimosa.
191
492240
2776
08:27
So I want you to recall what happens
192
495040
1736
08:28
when we touch the leaves of the mimosa.
193
496800
1936
08:30
It has touch receptors
that are sending that information
194
498760
2656
08:33
back down in the form
of an action potential.
195
501440
2096
08:35
And so what would happen
196
503560
1336
08:36
if we took the action potential
from the Venus flytrap
197
504920
3616
08:40
and sent it into
all the stems of the mimosa?
198
508560
3080
08:44
We should be able to create
the behavior of the mimosas
199
512440
3016
08:47
without actually touching it ourselves.
200
515480
1896
08:49
And so if you'll allow me,
201
517400
2016
08:51
I'm going to go ahead
and trigger this mimosa right now
202
519440
3455
08:54
by touching on the hairs
of the Venus flytrap.
203
522919
3817
08:58
So we're going to send information
about touch from one plant to another.
204
526760
3600
09:06
So there you see it.
205
534640
1696
09:08
So --
206
536360
1216
09:09
(Applause)
207
537600
6016
09:15
So I hope you learned a little bit,
something about plants today,
208
543640
3056
09:18
and not only that.
209
546720
1216
09:19
You learned that plants could be used
to help teach neuroscience
210
547960
3016
09:23
and bring along the neurorevolution.
211
551000
1736
09:24
Thank you.
212
552760
1216
09:26
(Applause)
213
554000
2720

▲Back to top

ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Greg Gage - Neuroscientist
TED Fellow Greg Gage helps kids investigate the neuroscience in their own backyards.

Why you should listen

As half of Backyard Brains, neuroscientist and engineer Greg Gage builds the SpikerBox -- a small rig that helps kids understand the electrical impulses that control the nervous system. He's passionate about helping students understand (viscerally) how our brains and our neurons work, because, as he said onstage at TED2012, we still know very little about how the brain works -- and we need to start inspiring kids early to want to know more.

Before becoming a neuroscientist, Gage worked as an electrical engineer making touchscreens. As he told the Huffington Post: "Scientific equipment in general is pretty expensive, but it's silly because before [getting my PhD in neuroscience] I was an electrical engineer, and you could see that you could make it yourself. So we started as a way to have fun, to show off to our colleagues, but we were also going into classrooms around that time and we thought, wouldn't it be cool if you could bring these gadgets with us so the stuff we were doing in advanced Ph.D. programs in neuroscience, you could also do in fifth grade?" His latest pieces of gear: the Roboroach, a cockroach fitted with an electric backpack that makes it turn on command, and BYB SmartScope, a smartphone-powered microscope.

More profile about the speaker
Greg Gage | Speaker | TED.com

Data provided by TED.

This site was created in May 2015 and the last update was on January 12, 2020. It will no longer be updated.

We are currently creating a new site called "eng.lish.video" and would be grateful if you could access it.

If you have any questions or suggestions, please feel free to write comments in your language on the contact form.

Privacy Policy

Developer's Blog

Buy Me A Coffee