ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Tali Sharot - Cognitive neuroscientist
Tali Sharot studies why our brains are biased toward optimism.

Why you should listen

Optimism bias is the belief that the future will be better, much better, than the past or present. And most of us display this bias. Neuroscientist Tali Sharot wants to know why: What is it about our brains that makes us overestimate the positive? She explores the question in her book The Optimism Bias: A Tour of the Irrationally Positive Brain

In the book (and a 2011 TIME magazine cover story), she reviewed findings from both social science and neuroscience that point to an interesting conclusion: "our brains aren't just stamped by the past. They are constantly being shaped by the future." In her own work, she's interested in how our natural optimism actually shapes what we remember, and her interesting range of papers encompasses behavioral research (how likely we are to misremember major events) as well as medical findings -- like searching for the places in the brain where optimism lives. Sharot is a faculty member of the Department of Cognitive, Perceptual and Brain Sciences at University College London.

 

More profile about the speaker
Tali Sharot | Speaker | TED.com
TED2012

Tali Sharot: The optimism bias

Tali Sharot: 樂觀偏見

Filmed:
2,387,106 views

我們是否生來就樂觀,而非實際?Tali Sharot分享了新的研究建議我們的大腦應該看光明面─又為何是何既危險又有利
- Cognitive neuroscientist
Tali Sharot studies why our brains are biased toward optimism. Full bio

Double-click the English transcript below to play the video.

00:16
I'm going to talk to you about optimism樂觀 --
0
148
3382
我想和你們談談樂觀主義--
00:19
or more precisely恰恰, the optimism樂觀 bias偏壓.
1
3530
2483
或者更準確的說,樂觀偏見。
00:21
It's a cognitive認知 illusion錯覺
2
6013
1645
這是個認知錯覺
00:23
that we've我們已經 been studying研究 in my lab實驗室 for the past過去 few少數 years年份,
3
7658
2742
過去數年來一直在我的實驗室裏研究著,
00:26
and 80 percent百分 of us have it.
4
10400
1735
我們之中有80%都得過。
00:28
It's our tendency趨勢 to overestimate估計過高
5
12135
2734
我們容易高估
00:30
our likelihood可能性 of experiencing經歷 good events事件 in our lives生活
6
14869
2923
可能是因為生活中所經歷的都是好事
00:33
and underestimate低估 our likelihood可能性 of experiencing經歷 bad events事件.
7
17792
4108
卻低估了可能也會經歷到壞事
00:37
So we underestimate低估 our likelihood可能性 of suffering痛苦 from cancer癌症,
8
21900
3017
所以我們低估了可能也會患有癌症
00:40
being存在 in a car汽車 accident事故.
9
24917
1506
可能也會出車禍
00:42
We overestimate估計過高 our longevity長壽, our career事業 prospects前途.
10
26423
3186
我們高估了壽命、職涯前景
00:45
In short, we're more optimistic樂觀 than realistic實際,
11
29609
3445
簡言之,我們是樂觀多於實際
00:48
but we are oblivious渾然不覺 to the fact事實.
12
33054
2162
但是我們都忽略了一個事實
00:51
Take marriage婚姻 for example.
13
35216
2227
就拿婚姻來說
00:53
In the Western西 world世界, divorce離婚 rates利率 are about 40 percent百分.
14
37443
3534
離婚率在西方世界大約是40%
00:56
That means手段 that out of five married已婚 couples情侶,
15
40977
3469
也就是說五對已婚夫妻中
01:00
two will end結束 up splitting分裂 their assets資產.
16
44446
2721
有兩對會玩完了然後分財產
01:03
But when you ask newlyweds新婚夫婦 about their own擁有 likelihood可能性 of divorce離婚,
17
47167
3375
但是當你問新婚夫妻他們本身的離婚可能性時
01:06
they estimate估計 it at zero percent百分.
18
50542
3692
他們都說百分之百的不可能
01:10
And even divorce離婚 lawyers律師, who should really know better,
19
54234
3729
甚至連離婚律師,他才更應該真正的了解,
01:13
hugely巨大 underestimate低估 their own擁有 likelihood可能性 of divorce離婚.
20
57963
3849
也是大大地低估了他們自己離婚的可能性。
01:17
So it turns out that optimists樂觀主義者 are not less likely容易 to divorce離婚,
21
61812
3126
所以結論就是樂觀者不是不可能離婚
01:20
but they are more likely容易 to remarry再婚.
22
64938
2530
而是較可能再婚
01:23
In the words of Samuel塞繆爾 Johnson約翰遜,
23
67468
2303
塞謬耳‧約翰曾說過,
01:25
"Remarriage再婚 is the triumph勝利 of hope希望 over experience經驗."
24
69771
3997
“再婚是希望對經歷的勝利。”
01:29
(Laughter笑聲)
25
73768
1962
(觀眾大笑)
01:31
So if we're married已婚, we're more likely容易 to have kids孩子.
26
75730
4574
所以如果我們結了婚,就比較可能有小孩。
01:36
And we all think our kids孩子 will be especially特別 talented天才.
27
80304
3265
而且都會認為我們的小孩特別聰明。
01:39
This, by the way, is my two-year-old二十歲 nephew外甥, Guy.
28
83569
2598
這一位,順便一提,是我的兩歲外甥,蓋。
01:42
And I just want to make it absolutely絕對 clear明確
29
86167
2375
我想說的只是
01:44
that he's a really bad example of the optimism樂觀 bias偏壓,
30
88542
2771
他真的是樂觀偏見的壞榜樣
01:47
because he is in fact事實 uniquely獨特地 talented天才.
31
91313
2812
因為他真的是絕頂聰明。
01:50
(Laughter笑聲)
32
94125
1435
(觀眾大笑)
01:51
And I'm not alone單獨.
33
95560
1607
而且不是只有我這麼認為
01:53
Out of four British英國的 people, three said
34
97167
2475
四個英國人中,有三個會說
01:55
that they were optimistic樂觀 about the future未來 of their own擁有 families家庭.
35
99642
3739
他們對於自己家人的前途都相當樂觀
01:59
That's 75 percent百分.
36
103381
1751
這樣的比例有75%
02:01
But only 30 percent百分 said
37
105132
2058
但只有30%的人說
02:03
that they thought families家庭 in general一般
38
107190
2560
他們認為一般說來家人
02:05
are doing better than a few少數 generations ago.
39
109750
2412
做得比前幾代要好
02:08
And this is a really important重要 point,
40
112162
2021
這真的是很重要的一點,
02:10
because we're optimistic樂觀 about ourselves我們自己,
41
114183
1888
因為我們對自己樂觀,
02:11
we're optimistic樂觀 about our kids孩子,
42
116071
1818
我們對孩子樂觀,
02:13
we're optimistic樂觀 about our families家庭,
43
117889
1788
對我們的家人樂觀,
02:15
but we're not so optimistic樂觀 about the guy sitting坐在 next下一個 to us,
44
119677
3183
但是對坐在我們旁邊的人卻不是那麼樂觀,
02:18
and we're somewhat有些 pessimistic悲觀
45
122860
2229
反而有些悲觀
02:20
about the fate命運 of our fellow同伴 citizens公民 and the fate命運 of our country國家.
46
125089
3879
對我們的市民同胞的命運及國家的命運。
02:24
But private私人的 optimism樂觀 about our own擁有 personal個人 future未來
47
128968
4036
但是對於我們個人前途的私人樂觀
02:28
remains遺跡 persistent一貫.
48
133004
2011
仍然持續。
02:30
And it doesn't mean that we think things will magically神奇 turn out okay,
49
135015
4066
這並不是說我們認為事情會奇蹟般的變好,
02:34
but rather that we have the unique獨特 ability能力 to make it so.
50
139081
4032
而是我們有獨特的能力使它變好
02:39
Now I'm a scientist科學家, I do experiments實驗.
51
143113
2841
現在,我是個科學家,我做實驗。
02:41
So to show顯示 you what I mean,
52
145954
1792
所以為了告訴你們我的本意,
02:43
I'm going to do an experiment實驗 here with you.
53
147746
2802
我在這裏要和你們一起做個實驗。
02:46
So I'm going to give you a list名單 of abilities能力 and characteristics特點,
54
150548
3850
我打算列出一張能力和特性的清單,
02:50
and I want you to think for each of these abilities能力
55
154398
2894
我要你們想想看這些每個能力
02:53
where you stand relative相對的 to the rest休息 of the population人口.
56
157292
4393
你所佔據的位置,與其它人相比
02:57
The first one is getting得到 along沿 well with others其他.
57
161685
3786
第一個是和他人相處得很好
03:01
Who here believes相信 they're at the bottom底部 25 percent百分?
58
165471
5652
在座各位有誰相信自己是最底部的25%?
03:07
Okay, that's about 10 people out of 1,500.
59
171123
4322
好吧,那大概是1,500人中的10個
03:11
Who believes相信 they're at the top最佳 25 percent百分?
60
175445
4013
有誰相信自己是最頂端的25%?
03:15
That's most of us here.
61
179458
2710
這裏大部份人都相信。
03:18
Okay, now do the same相同 for your driving主動 ability能力.
62
182168
4930
好。 同樣的事也發生在你的駕駛能力上
03:22
How interesting有趣 are you?
63
187098
2583
你這個人有多有趣?
03:25
How attractive有吸引力 are you?
64
189681
2844
你有多少吸引力?
03:28
How honest誠實 are you?
65
192525
2954
你有多誠實?
03:31
And finally最後, how modest謙虛 are you?
66
195479
5000
最後,你有多謙虛?
03:36
So most of us put ourselves我們自己 above以上 average平均
67
200479
2688
所以大部份的人把自己放在水平之上
03:39
on most of these abilities能力.
68
203167
2029
對這些能力來說
03:41
Now this is statistically統計學 impossible不可能.
69
205196
2131
這是完全不可能的
03:43
We can't all be better than everyone大家 else其他.
70
207327
3229
我們不可能次次比別人好
03:46
(Laughter笑聲)
71
210556
1642
(觀眾大笑)
03:48
But if we believe we're better than the other guy,
72
212198
2677
但是如果我們相信自己比他人好
03:50
well that means手段 that we're more likely容易 to get that promotion提升, to remain married已婚,
73
214875
4083
就是說我們容易高升、容易維持婚姻
03:54
because we're more social社會, more interesting有趣.
74
218958
2598
因為我們較社會化、較有趣
03:57
And it's a global全球 phenomenon現象.
75
221556
2163
這是個全球化的現象
03:59
The optimism樂觀 bias偏壓 has been observed觀察到的
76
223719
2468
樂觀偏見已經被注意到了
04:02
in many許多 different不同 countries國家 --
77
226187
1719
在許多不同的國家裏
04:03
in Western西 cultures文化, in non-Western非西方 cultures文化,
78
227906
3104
在西方文化裏、在非西方文化裏
04:06
in females女性 and males男性,
79
231010
1506
在女性與男性之間
04:08
in kids孩子, in the elderly老年.
80
232516
1724
在小孩和年長者之間
04:10
It's quite相當 widespread廣泛.
81
234240
1783
分佈甚廣
04:11
But the question is, is it good for us?
82
236023
3500
但問題是,對我們而言好嗎?
04:15
So some people say no.
83
239523
2454
有些人說不好
04:17
Some people say the secret秘密 to happiness幸福
84
241977
2518
有些人說幸福的秘訣
04:20
is low expectations期望.
85
244495
2753
就是期望不高
04:23
I think the logic邏輯 goes something like this:
86
247248
2504
我想邏輯是這樣的:
04:25
If we don't expect期望 greatness偉大,
87
249752
2258
如果我們的期盼不高,
04:27
if we don't expect期望 to find love and be healthy健康 and successful成功,
88
252010
3880
如果我們不期盼找到愛、身體健康和事業成功,
04:31
well we're not going to be disappointed失望 when these things don't happen發生.
89
255890
3098
嗯,當這些事都沒發生時,我們就不會失望。
04:34
And if we're not disappointed失望 when good things don't happen發生,
90
258988
3408
而且,就算好事沒發生時,我們也不會失望
04:38
and we're pleasantly愉快 surprised詫異 when they do,
91
262396
1979
如果發生了好事,就算給我們一個大驚喜
04:40
we will be happy快樂.
92
264375
1781
我們會很快樂。
04:42
So it's a very good theory理論,
93
266156
1702
所以這是個相當好的推論,
04:43
but it turns out to be wrong錯誤 for three reasons原因.
94
267858
3027
但是有三個理由讓它變成錯的。
04:46
Number one: Whatever隨你 happens發生, whether是否 you succeed成功 or you fail失敗,
95
270885
4615
第一:不論發生什麼事,不管你成功還是失敗,
04:51
people with high expectations期望 always feel better.
96
275500
3858
期待高的人通常都會自我感覺較好。
04:55
Because how we feel when we get dumped or win贏得 employee僱員 of the month
97
279358
3932
因為,我們被人拋棄或當上明星店員的感覺如何,
04:59
depends依靠 on how we interpret that event事件.
98
283290
3030
完全看我們怎麼解釋那件事而定
05:02
The psychologists心理學家 Margaret瑪格麗特 Marshall馬歇爾 and John約翰 Brown棕色
99
286320
4050
心裡學家Margraret Marshall 及 John Brown
05:06
studied研究 students學生們 with high and low expectations期望.
100
290370
2994
研究期盼高及期盼低的學生。
05:09
And they found發現 that when people with high expectations期望 succeed成功,
101
293364
4184
他發現期待高的人成功時,
05:13
they attribute屬性 that success成功 to their own擁有 traits性狀.
102
297548
2910
他們將成功歸功於自己的人格特質。
05:16
"I'm a genius天才, therefore因此 I got an A,
103
300458
2196
’“我是個天才,所以我得個A,
05:18
therefore因此 I'll get an A again and again in the future未來."
104
302654
2675
所以以後我也會一再得A。”
05:21
When they failed失敗, it wasn't because they were dumb,
105
305329
2973
當他們失敗時,不是因為他們笨。
05:24
but because the exam考試 just happened發生 to be unfair不公平.
106
308302
3173
而是因為這個考試剛巧不公平,
05:27
Next下一個 time they will do better.
107
311475
2283
下一次他們會考得更好。
05:29
People with low expectations期望 do the opposite對面.
108
313758
3077
期盼低的人剛好相反。
05:32
So when they failed失敗 it was because they were dumb,
109
316835
2832
所以當他們失敗時他們就是笨,
05:35
and when they succeeded成功
110
319667
1500
而當他們成功時,
05:37
it was because the exam考試 just happened發生 to be really easy簡單.
111
321167
3317
是因為考試剛巧真的太簡單,
05:40
Next下一個 time reality現實 would catch抓住 up with them.
112
324484
2401
下一次就會露出馬腳了。
05:42
So they felt worse更差.
113
326885
2073
所以他們覺得很糟。
05:44
Number two: Regardless而不管 of the outcome結果,
114
328958
3240
第二:不管結果,
05:48
the pure act法案 of anticipation預期 makes品牌 us happy快樂.
115
332198
4047
單純的期待行為使我們快樂,
05:52
The behavioral行為的 economist經濟學家 George喬治 Lowenstein洛溫斯坦
116
336245
3176
行為經濟學家George Lowenstein
05:55
asked students學生們 in his university大學
117
339421
1719
在大學裏問學生
05:57
to imagine想像 getting得到 a passionate多情 kiss from a celebrity名人, any celebrity名人.
118
341140
4362
得到名人的熱吻的印象,任何名人
06:01
Then he said, "How much are you willing願意 to pay工資
119
345502
2885
他說:“你願付多少錢
06:04
to get a kiss from a celebrity名人
120
348387
1988
得到名人的吻
06:06
if the kiss was delivered交付 immediately立即,
121
350375
2227
如果吻馬上就來,
06:08
in three hours小時, in 24 hours小時, in three days,
122
352602
5023
3小時內,24小時內,3天內
06:13
in one year, in 10 years年份?
123
357625
2433
1年內,10年內?
06:15
He found發現 that the students學生們 were willing願意 to pay工資 the most
124
360058
3130
他發現學生最願意付的是
06:19
not to get a kiss immediately立即,
125
363188
1999
不要馬上就能得到的吻
06:21
but to get a kiss in three days.
126
365187
2980
而是3天內能得到的吻,
06:24
They were willing願意 to pay工資 extra額外 in order訂購 to wait.
127
368167
4247
也有人願意支付額外費用來等待一個吻
06:28
Now they weren't willing願意 to wait a year or 10 years年份;
128
372414
3003
但他們不願意一等就等個1年或10年的;
06:31
no one wants an aging老化 celebrity名人.
129
375417
1421
因為沒人會要個上了年紀的名人,
06:32
But three days seemed似乎 to be the optimum最佳 amount.
130
376838
4789
但是三天似乎是最佳的天數
06:37
So why is that?
131
381627
2303
為什麼會這樣?
06:39
Well if you get the kiss now, it's over and doneDONE with.
132
383930
3424
嗯。如果你現在馬上就被親一下,那一切就沒事了
06:43
But if you get the kiss in three days,
133
387354
1950
但是如果你三天內才被親
06:45
well that's three days of jittery風聲鶴唳 anticipation預期, the thrill顫抖 of the wait.
134
389304
4154
那就有三天緊張的預期,等待的快感
06:49
The students學生們 wanted that time
135
393458
1869
學生要那個時間
06:51
to imagine想像 where is it going to happen發生,
136
395327
2381
來想像即將要發生什麼事,
06:53
how is it going to happen發生.
137
397708
1629
究竟是怎麼發生的。
06:55
Anticipation預期 made製作 them happy快樂.
138
399337
2211
期待使他們快樂。
06:57
This is, by the way, why people prefer比較喜歡 Friday星期五 to Sunday星期日.
139
401548
3869
這也就是,順道一提的,為何人們比週日還喜歡週五。
07:01
It's a really curious好奇 fact事實,
140
405417
2083
這真是讓人難以理解的事實,
07:03
because Friday星期五 is a day of work and Sunday星期日 is a day of pleasure樂趣,
141
407500
3417
因為週五是個工作天而週日是個歡樂天,
07:06
so you'd assume承擔 that people will prefer比較喜歡 Sunday星期日,
142
410917
2904
所以可以假設人們較喜歡星期天,
07:09
but they don't.
143
413821
1721
但不是。
07:11
It's not because they really, really like being存在 in the office辦公室
144
415542
2875
不是因為他們真的、真的愛呆在辦公室
07:14
and they can't stand strolling漫步 in the park公園
145
418417
1967
也不是因為他們不能忍受在公園裏散步
07:16
or having a lazy brunch早午餐.
146
420384
1661
或是不想吃個慵懶的早午餐
07:17
We know that, because when you ask people
147
422045
2136
我們知道,因為當你問人
07:20
about their ultimate最終 favorite喜愛 day of the week,
148
424181
2704
一週中最喜歡哪一天
07:22
surprise, surprise, Saturday星期六 comes in at first,
149
426885
2820
驚訝吧,驚訝吧,週六是首選,
07:25
then Friday星期五, then Sunday星期日.
150
429705
2920
之後是週五,再來才是週日,
07:28
People prefer比較喜歡 Friday星期五
151
432625
1869
人們喜歡週五
07:30
because Friday星期五 brings帶來 with it the anticipation預期 of the weekend週末 ahead,
152
434494
3964
因為週五為週末帶來期待,
07:34
all the plans計劃 that you have.
153
438458
1917
期待所有的計劃
07:36
On Sunday星期日, the only thing you can look forward前鋒 to
154
440375
2704
但在週日,唯一能企望到的
07:38
is the work week.
155
443079
2254
就是整週的工作。
07:41
So optimists樂觀主義者 are people who expect期望 more kisses in their future未來,
156
445333
4877
所以樂觀者就是期待未來能有更多吻的人
07:46
more strolls散步 in the park公園.
157
450210
1921
在公園裏更多的散步
07:48
And that anticipation預期 enhances提高 their wellbeing福利.
158
452131
3907
所以期待提高了他們的幸福感
07:51
In fact事實, without the optimism樂觀 bias偏壓,
159
456038
2964
事實上,沒有了樂觀偏見
07:54
we would all be slightly depressed鬱悶.
160
459002
2750
我們都會有一點點沮喪
07:57
People with mild溫和 depression蕭條,
161
461752
2267
輕度憂鬱症患者,
07:59
they don't have a bias偏壓 when they look into the future未來.
162
464019
2979
當他們展望未來時完全沒有偏見
08:02
They're actually其實 more realistic實際 than healthy健康 individuals個人.
163
466998
4222
實際上,他們比健康的人更現實
08:07
But individuals個人 with severe嚴重 depression蕭條,
164
471220
2101
但是重度憂鬱症患者
08:09
they have a pessimistic悲觀 bias偏壓.
165
473321
1829
他們有悲觀的偏見
08:11
So they tend趨向 to expect期望 the future未來
166
475150
2463
所以往往盼望前途
08:13
to be worse更差 than it ends結束 up being存在.
167
477613
2477
比死亡結束時要差
08:15
So optimism樂觀 changes變化 subjective主觀 reality現實.
168
480090
3243
所以樂觀改變了主觀的現實
08:19
The way we expect期望 the world世界 to be changes變化 the way we see it.
169
483333
3750
我們期盼世界變成我們眼見的方式
08:22
But it also changes變化 objective目的 reality現實.
170
487083
3242
但是也改變了客觀存在
08:26
It acts行為 as a self-fulfilling自我實現 prophecy預言.
171
490325
2727
它表現得像個自我實現的預言
08:28
And that is the third第三 reason原因
172
493052
2068
第三個理由
08:31
why lowering降低 your expectations期望 will not make you happy快樂.
173
495120
3180
為什麼降低期待不會使你感到快樂
08:34
Controlled受控 experiments實驗 have shown顯示
174
498300
1835
對照實驗顯示
08:36
that optimism樂觀 is not only related有關 to success成功,
175
500135
2917
樂觀不只和成功有關,
08:38
it leads引線 to success成功.
176
503052
1807
樂觀導向成功
08:40
Optimism樂觀 leads引線 to success成功 in academia學術界 and sports體育 and politics政治.
177
504859
4870
樂觀在學術界及運動界和政治界導向成功
08:45
And maybe the most surprising奇怪 benefit效益 of optimism樂觀 is health健康.
178
509729
4475
樂觀最令人驚喜的好處就是健康
08:50
If we expect期望 the future未來 to be bright,
179
514204
3325
如果我們期盼前途是光明的
08:53
stress強調 and anxiety焦慮 are reduced減少.
180
517529
2740
壓力及焦慮會降低
08:56
So all in all, optimism樂觀 has lots of benefits好處.
181
520269
4220
所以總而言之,樂觀有很多好處
09:00
But the question that was really confusing撲朔迷離 to me was,
182
524489
3011
但是真正困擾我的問題是
09:03
how do we maintain保持 optimism樂觀 in the face面對 of reality現實?
183
527500
4669
我們如何在現實面前保持樂觀?
09:08
As an neuroscientist神經學家, this was especially特別 confusing撲朔迷離,
184
532169
3018
身為一個神經科學家,感到特別的困擾,
09:11
because according根據 to all the theories理論 out there,
185
535187
2896
因為根據那裏所有的理論
09:13
when your expectations期望 are not met會見, you should alter改變 them.
186
538083
4188
期待未達時,就應該改變他們
09:18
But this is not what we find.
187
542271
1552
但這不是我們所發現的
09:19
We asked people to come into our lab實驗室
188
543823
3483
我們要求人們來到實驗室
09:23
in order訂購 to try and figure數字 out what was going on.
189
547306
2810
為的是嘗試而且弄清楚倒底是怎麼一回事
09:26
We asked them to estimate估計 their likelihood可能性
190
550116
2547
我們要求他們評估自己
09:28
of experiencing經歷 different不同 terrible可怕 events事件 in their lives生活.
191
552663
2504
經歷生活中不同的恐怖事件的可能性
09:31
So, for example, what is your likelihood可能性 of suffering痛苦 from cancer癌症?
192
555167
4375
例如,得癌症的可能性為何?
09:35
And then we told them the average平均 likelihood可能性
193
559542
2352
之後我們告訴他們像他們這樣的人
09:37
of someone有人 like them to suffer遭受 these misfortunes不幸.
194
561894
2981
遭受這些不幸的平均可能性
09:40
So cancer癌症, for example, is about 30 percent百分.
195
564875
3565
所以癌症,舉例來說,大約30%
09:44
And then we asked them again,
196
568440
2935
之後我們再問他們一次,
09:47
"How likely容易 are you to suffer遭受 from cancer癌症?"
197
571375
2875
“得癌症的可能性有多大?”
09:50
What we wanted to know was
198
574250
2248
我們想知道的是
09:52
whether是否 people will take the information信息 that we gave them
199
576498
2669
人們是否會採信我們給的消息
09:55
to change更改 their beliefs信仰.
200
579167
2131
來改變他們的看法。
09:57
And indeed確實 they did --
201
581298
2369
他們真的會
09:59
but mostly大多 when the information信息 we gave them
202
583667
2581
但大部份是在我們給他們的消息
10:02
was better than what they expected預期.
203
586248
2831
比他們期待的消息還要好的時候
10:04
So for example,
204
589079
1361
所以舉例來說
10:06
if someone有人 said, "My likelihood可能性 of suffering痛苦 from cancer癌症
205
590440
2910
如果某人說,“我得癌症的可能性
10:09
is about 50 percent百分,"
206
593350
2381
大概是50%,”
10:11
and we said, "Hey, good news新聞.
207
595731
2521
我們會說,“嘿,好消息。
10:14
The average平均 likelihood可能性 is only 30 percent百分,"
208
598252
2581
平均可能性只有30%,”
10:16
the next下一個 time around they would say,
209
600833
2046
下一次大概他們就會說,
10:18
"Well maybe my likelihood可能性 is about 35 percent百分."
210
602879
2715
“嗯,我的可能性大約是35%,”
10:21
So they learned學到了 quickly很快 and efficiently有效率的.
211
605594
2636
所以他們學得很快也很有效率
10:24
But if someone有人 started開始 off saying,
212
608230
2550
但是如果有人開始說
10:26
"My average平均 likelihood可能性 of suffering痛苦 from cancer癌症 is about 10 percent百分,"
213
610780
3403
“我得癌的一般可能性大約是10%,”
10:30
and we said, "Hey, bad news新聞.
214
614183
2692
我們就會說“嗨,壞消息。
10:32
The average平均 likelihood可能性 is about 30 percent百分,"
215
616875
3073
一般得病的可能性是30%
10:35
the next下一個 time around they would say,
216
619948
2058
到了下一次大概他們會說
10:37
"Yep是的. Still think it's about 11 percent百分."
217
622006
3119
是,我仍然認為大約是11%
10:41
(Laughter笑聲)
218
625125
1690
(全場觀眾大笑)
10:42
So it's not that they didn't learn學習 at all -- they did --
219
626815
3319
所以不是他們完全不知道--他們知道--
10:46
but much, much less than when we gave them
220
630134
2336
但是比我們告訴他們的
10:48
positive information信息 about the future未來.
221
632470
2473
有關未來的正面信息要少很多
10:50
And it's not that they didn't remember記得 the numbers數字 that we gave them;
222
634943
3034
也不是他們不記得我們給他們的數字
10:53
everyone大家 remembers記得 that the average平均 likelihood可能性 of cancer癌症
223
637977
3073
每個人都記得癌症的平均可能性
10:56
is about 30 percent百分
224
641050
1500
大約是30%
10:58
and the average平均 likelihood可能性 of divorce離婚 is about 40 percent百分.
225
642550
2779
而離婚的平均可能性大約是40%
11:01
But they didn't think that those numbers數字 were related有關 to them.
226
645329
4631
但是他們不認為這些數字跟他們有關
11:05
What this means手段 is that warning警告 signs跡象 such這樣 as these
227
649960
4148
這表示像這樣的警告跡象
11:10
may可能 only have limited有限 impact碰撞.
228
654108
2892
影響也有限
11:12
Yes, smoking抽煙 kills殺死, but mostly大多 it kills殺死 the other guy.
229
657000
4159
是的,吸煙殺人,但是大部份是殺了別人
11:17
What I wanted to know was
230
661159
1883
我想知道的是
11:18
what was going on inside the human人的 brain
231
663042
2860
人類腦中倒底發生了什麼事
11:21
that prevented防止 us from taking服用 these warning警告 signs跡象 personally親自.
232
665902
4094
才會讓我們以為這些警告跡象不是衝著我們來的
11:25
But at the same相同 time,
233
669996
1487
但是同時
11:27
when we hear that the housing住房 market市場 is hopeful有希望,
234
671483
1978
當我們聽到房市很有希望的時候
11:29
we think, "Oh, my house is definitely無疑 going to double in price價錢."
235
673461
4116
我們就會想,“喔,我的房子絕對漲了兩倍
11:33
To try and figure數字 that out,
236
677577
2085
試著找出來
11:35
I asked the participants參與者 in the experiment實驗
237
679662
2473
我要求實驗室裏的參與者
11:38
to lie謊言 in a brain imaging成像 scanner掃描器.
238
682135
2161
躺在腦成像掃描儀裏
11:40
It looks容貌 like this.
239
684296
1527
看起來就像這樣
11:41
And using運用 a method方法 called functional實用 MRIMRI,
240
685823
3031
而且用一種叫做核磁共振的儀器
11:44
we were able能夠 to identify鑑定 regions地區 in the brain
241
688854
3606
我們可以辨認出腦中的哪些部位
11:48
that were responding響應 to positive information信息.
242
692460
2817
回應了正面信息
11:51
One of these regions地區 is called the left inferior frontal前面的 gyrus.
243
695277
3579
其中一個部份被稱為左前下腦回
11:54
So if someone有人 said, "My likelihood可能性 of suffering痛苦 from cancer癌症 is 50 percent百分,"
244
698856
3788
所以如果某人說,“我得癌的可能性是50%”
11:58
and we said, "Hey, good news新聞.
245
702644
1856
我們會說,“嘿,好消息。”
12:00
Average平均 likelihood可能性 is 30 percent百分,"
246
704500
2290
一般的可能性是30%”
12:02
the left inferior frontal前面的 gyrus would respond響應 fiercely激烈.
247
706790
3572
左前下腦回將強烈的回應
12:06
And it didn't matter if you're an extreme極端 optimist樂天派, a mild溫和 optimist樂天派
248
710362
4732
就算你是個極端樂觀主義者,溫和的樂觀主義者,
12:10
or slightly pessimistic悲觀,
249
715094
1821
或是輕度的悲觀者 ,那都不重要
12:12
everyone's大家的 left inferior frontal前面的 gyrus
250
716915
2585
每個人的左前下腦回
12:15
was functioning功能 perfectly完美 well,
251
719500
1844
都運作得很好
12:17
whether是否 you're Barack巴拉克 Obama奧巴馬 or Woody伍迪 Allen艾倫.
252
721344
2906
不管你是歐巴馬或是伍迪艾倫
12:20
On the other side of the brain,
253
724250
1644
在腦的另一邊
12:21
the right inferior frontal前面的 gyrus was responding響應 to bad news新聞.
254
725894
4786
右前下腦回只對壞消息有所回應
12:26
And here's這裡的 the thing: it wasn't doing a very good job工作.
255
730680
3726
事情來了: 它做得不是很好
12:30
The more optimistic樂觀 you were,
256
734406
2015
你愈是樂觀
12:32
the less likely容易 this region地區 was
257
736421
2337
這個部份就不太可能
12:34
to respond響應 to unexpected意外 negative information信息.
258
738758
2982
對意想不到的負面消息做出回應
12:37
And if your brain is failing失敗
259
741740
3058
如果你的大腦
12:40
at integrating整合 bad news新聞 about the future未來,
260
744798
3225
無法整合與未來有關的壞消息
12:43
you will constantly經常 leave離開 your rose-tinted玫瑰色 spectacles眼鏡 on.
261
748023
4623
你就會一直保持樂觀
12:48
So we wanted to know, could we change更改 this?
262
752646
4969
所以我們想知道,我們能改變嗎?
12:53
Could we alter改變 people's人們 optimism樂觀 bias偏壓
263
757615
3443
我們能不能透過干擾這些部份的腦活動
12:56
by interfering干擾 with the brain activity活動 in these regions地區?
264
761058
3932
來改變人們的樂觀偏見?
13:00
And there's a way for us to do that.
265
764990
2595
有一個方法
13:03
This is my collaborator合作者 Ryota良太 Kanai金井.
266
767585
2627
這是我的合作人Ryota Kanai
13:06
And what he's doing is he's passing通過 a small magnetic磁性 pulse脈衝
267
770212
3955
他正在做的就是把小型電磁脈衝
13:10
through通過 the skull頭骨 of the participant參加者 in our study研究
268
774167
2119
透過參加研究者的頭骨
13:12
into their inferior frontal前面的 gyrus.
269
776286
2245
傳送到他們的額下回
13:14
And by doing that,
270
778531
1927
這樣做
13:16
he's interfering干擾 with the activity活動 of this brain region地區
271
780458
2904
他的這些大腦區域活動就會被干擾
13:19
for about half an hour小時.
272
783362
1215
大約半小時
13:20
After that everything goes back to normal正常, I assure保證 you.
273
784577
2921
我向你保證,之後每件事都回到正常
13:23
(Laughter笑聲)
274
787498
1993
(觀眾大笑)
13:25
So let's see what happens發生.
275
789491
3154
所以讓我們看看發生了什麼事
13:28
First of all, I'm going to show顯示 you
276
792645
2097
首先,我要給你們看的是
13:30
the average平均 amount of bias偏壓 that we see.
277
794742
2704
我們所見的偏見的平均量
13:33
So if I was to test測試 all of you now,
278
797446
2823
所以如果現在我要試試你們所有的人
13:36
this is the amount that you would learn學習
279
800269
1794
這就是你們
13:37
more from good news新聞 relative相對的 to bad news新聞.
280
802063
3120
相對於壞消息來說 聽到了更多好消息
13:41
Now we interfere干擾 with the region地區
281
805183
2484
現在我們來干擾一下
13:43
that we found發現 to integrate整合 negative information信息 in this task任務,
282
807667
4467
我們找到的這個 整合負面消息的腦部區域
13:48
and the optimism樂觀 bias偏壓 grew成長 even larger.
283
812134
3564
樂觀主義的偏見就會愈來愈大
13:51
We made製作 people more biased in the way that they process處理 information信息.
284
815698
5470
我們使得人們在處理消息的方面更加偏激
13:57
Then we interfered干擾 with the brain region地區
285
821168
2897
所以我們干擾腦區域
13:59
that we found發現 to integrate整合 good news新聞 in this task任務,
286
824065
3518
我們找到的這個 整合負面消息的腦部區域
14:03
and the optimism樂觀 bias偏壓 disappeared消失.
287
827583
4471
而樂觀偏見就消失了
14:07
We were quite相當 amazed吃驚 by these results結果
288
832054
2008
我們對這些結果相當震驚
14:09
because we were able能夠 to eliminate消除
289
834062
2192
因為我們可以消除
14:12
a deep-rooted刻骨 bias偏壓 in humans人類.
290
836254
3196
人類根深蒂固的偏見
14:15
And at this point we stopped停止 and we asked ourselves我們自己,
291
839450
4773
在這一點上我們停了下來而且問自己
14:20
would we want to shatter打碎 the optimism樂觀 illusion錯覺 into tiny little bits?
292
844223
5102
是不是要把樂觀的幻想粉碎成一小片一小片?
14:25
If we could do that, would we want to take people's人們 optimism樂觀 bias偏壓 away?
293
849325
5044
如果可以這樣做,我們會不會是想取走人們的樂觀偏見?
14:30
Well I've already已經 told you about all of the benefits好處 of the optimism樂觀 bias偏壓,
294
854369
4600
我已經告訴你樂觀偏見的所有好處
14:34
which哪一個 probably大概 makes品牌 you want to hold保持 onto it for dear life.
295
858969
4321
你可能會想在寶貴的生活裏繼續保有
14:39
But there are, of course課程, pitfalls陷阱,
296
863290
2085
但是當然有圈套
14:41
and it would be really foolish of us to ignore忽視 them.
297
865375
3120
而我們真是蠢到忽略了他們
14:44
Take for example this email電子郵件 I recieved收到
298
868495
3546
舉個例子來說,我從一位加州的消防隊員
14:47
from a firefighter消防隊員 here in California加州.
299
872041
2694
收到這封電子郵件
14:50
He says, "Fatality死亡事故 investigations調查 for firefighters消防員
300
874735
3304
他說,“消防隊員的死亡調查
14:53
often經常 include包括 'We'我們 didn't think the fire was going to do that,'
301
878039
4142
通常包括“我們不認為火勢會像這樣
14:58
even when all of the available可得到 information信息
302
882181
2302
即使所有現有的消息
15:00
was there to make safe安全 decisions決定."
303
884483
2255
都是安全的
15:02
This captain隊長 is going to use our findings發現 on the optimism樂觀 bias偏壓
304
886738
3874
這位船長想使用我們在樂觀偏見上的研究結果
15:06
to try to explain說明 to the firefighters消防員
305
890612
2192
試著向消防隊員解釋
15:08
why they think the way they do,
306
892804
2209
為什麼他們會認為他們做事的方法
15:10
to make them acutely急性 aware知道的 of this very optimistic樂觀 bias偏壓 in humans人類.
307
895013
7126
會使得他們敏銳地意識到人類的樂觀偏見
15:18
So unrealistic不切實際 optimism樂觀 can lead to risky有風險 behavior行為,
308
902139
5119
所以不切實際的樂觀會導致危險的行為
15:23
to financial金融 collapse坍方, to faulty不完善的 planning規劃.
309
907258
3638
導致金融崩潰,導致計劃錯誤
15:26
The British英國的 government政府, for example,
310
910896
2138
舉例來說,英國政府
15:28
has acknowledged承認 that the optimism樂觀 bias偏壓
311
913034
2674
確認樂觀偏見
15:31
can make individuals個人 more likely容易
312
915708
3287
使得人們更有可能
15:34
to underestimate低估 the costs成本 and durations持續時間 of projects項目.
313
918995
4026
低估了計劃的成本及持續期間
15:38
So they have adjusted調整 the 2012 Olympic奧林匹克 budget預算
314
923021
4254
所以他們調整了2012年奧林匹克的預算
15:43
for the optimism樂觀 bias偏壓.
315
927275
2111
就是因為樂觀偏見
15:45
My friend朋友 who's誰是 getting得到 married已婚 in a few少數 weeks
316
929386
2187
我有一個將在幾星期內結婚的朋友
15:47
has doneDONE the same相同 for his wedding婚禮 budget預算.
317
931573
2434
也對結婚預算做了同樣的事
15:49
And by the way, when I asked him about his own擁有 likelihood可能性 of divorce離婚,
318
934007
3062
順便一提的是,當我問他自己離婚的可能性時
15:52
he said he was quite相當 sure it was zero percent百分.
319
937069
3560
他說他很確定是百分之零(完全不可能)
15:56
So what we would really like to do,
320
940629
2417
所以我們真正想做的
15:58
is we would like to protect保護 ourselves我們自己 from the dangers危險 of optimism樂觀,
321
943046
4161
是我們要保護自己免除樂觀危險
16:03
but at the same相同 time remain hopeful有希望,
322
947207
3228
但同時保持深具希望
16:06
benefiting受益 from the many許多 fruits水果 of optimism樂觀.
323
950435
2844
從樂觀的許多成果中獲益
16:09
And I believe there's a way for us to do that.
324
953279
2804
我相信一定有條路讓我們這樣做
16:11
The key here really is knowledge知識.
325
956083
2060
真正的關鍵是知識
16:14
We're not born天生 with an innate先天 understanding理解 of our biases偏見.
326
958143
3311
我們不是生來就了解偏見
16:17
These have to be identified確定 by scientific科學 investigation調查.
327
961454
3841
這些必需要透過科學調查來釐清
16:21
But the good news新聞 is that becoming變得 aware知道的 of the optimism樂觀 bias偏壓
328
965295
4184
但是好消息是知道了樂觀偏見
16:25
does not shatter打碎 the illusion錯覺.
329
969479
1775
並未粉碎幻想
16:27
It's like visual視覺 illusions幻想,
330
971254
1548
它就像是視覺上的錯覺
16:28
in which哪一個 understanding理解 them does not make them go away.
331
972802
3490
了解它們而非叫他們走
16:32
And this is good because it means手段
332
976292
2237
這是很好的因為意思是
16:34
we should be able能夠 to strike罷工 a balance平衡,
333
978529
2473
我們應該能夠找到折衷辦法
16:36
to come up with plans計劃 and rules規則
334
981002
2427
想出計劃和規則
16:39
to protect保護 ourselves我們自己 from unrealistic不切實際 optimism樂觀,
335
983429
2783
保護我們自己遠離不切實際的樂觀
16:42
but at the same相同 time remain hopeful有希望.
336
986212
3152
但同時保持深具希望
16:45
I think this cartoon動畫片 portrays刻畫 it nicely很好.
337
989364
3424
我認為這個卡通畫面很不錯
16:48
Because if you're one of these pessimistic悲觀 penguins企鵝 up there
338
992788
3526
因為如果你是站在那邊的悲觀企鵝的一員
16:52
who just does not believe they can fly,
339
996314
2119
就是不相信自己會飛
16:54
you certainly當然 never will.
340
998433
2359
當然就永遠也不會飛
16:56
Because to make any kind of progress進展,
341
1000792
1831
因為任何的進展
16:58
we need to be able能夠 to imagine想像 a different不同 reality現實,
342
1002623
2350
都需要想像一個不同的現實
17:00
and then we need to believe that that reality現實 is possible可能.
343
1004973
3731
之後我們必需相信那個現實是可能的
17:04
But if you are an extreme極端 optimistic樂觀 penguin企鵝
344
1008714
3524
但如果你是個極端樂觀的企鵝
17:08
who just jumps跳躍 down blindly盲目地 hoping希望 for the best最好,
345
1012238
2762
只是盲目的往下跳希望成為最好的
17:10
you might威力 find yourself你自己 in a bit of a mess食堂 when you hit擊中 the ground地面.
346
1015000
4781
當撞到地面的時候,你會發現有點麻煩
17:15
But if you're an optimistic樂觀 penguin企鵝
347
1019781
1889
但如果你是個樂觀的企鵝
17:17
who believes相信 they can fly,
348
1021670
1796
相信自己可以飛
17:19
but then adjusts調整 a parachute降落傘 to your back
349
1023466
2663
但之後又調整背後的降落傘
17:22
just in case案件 things don't work out exactly究竟 as you had planned計劃,
350
1026129
3017
萬一事情不完全按照你的計劃走
17:25
you will soar翱翔 like an eagle,
351
1029146
1863
你也可能像隻鷹一樣的翱翔
17:26
even if you're just a penguin企鵝.
352
1031009
3049
即使你只是隻企鵝
17:29
Thank you.
353
1034058
1762
謝謝你
17:31
(Applause掌聲)
354
1035820
2936
(觀眾鼓掌)
Translated by Shin-Yuan Tzou
Reviewed by Chen-Han Hsiao

▲Back to top

ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Tali Sharot - Cognitive neuroscientist
Tali Sharot studies why our brains are biased toward optimism.

Why you should listen

Optimism bias is the belief that the future will be better, much better, than the past or present. And most of us display this bias. Neuroscientist Tali Sharot wants to know why: What is it about our brains that makes us overestimate the positive? She explores the question in her book The Optimism Bias: A Tour of the Irrationally Positive Brain

In the book (and a 2011 TIME magazine cover story), she reviewed findings from both social science and neuroscience that point to an interesting conclusion: "our brains aren't just stamped by the past. They are constantly being shaped by the future." In her own work, she's interested in how our natural optimism actually shapes what we remember, and her interesting range of papers encompasses behavioral research (how likely we are to misremember major events) as well as medical findings -- like searching for the places in the brain where optimism lives. Sharot is a faculty member of the Department of Cognitive, Perceptual and Brain Sciences at University College London.

 

More profile about the speaker
Tali Sharot | Speaker | TED.com

Data provided by TED.

This site was created in May 2015 and the last update was on January 12, 2020. It will no longer be updated.

We are currently creating a new site called "eng.lish.video" and would be grateful if you could access it.

If you have any questions or suggestions, please feel free to write comments in your language on the contact form.

Privacy Policy

Developer's Blog

Buy Me A Coffee