ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Shlomo Benartzi - Economist
Shlomo Benartzi uses behavioral economics to study how and why we plan well for the future (or fail to), and uses that to develop new programs to encourage saving for retirement.

Why you should listen

Shlomo Benartzi studies behavioral finance with a special interest in personal finance. He is co-founder of the Behavioral Finance Forum (www.behavioralfinanceforum.com), a collective of 40 prominent academics and 40 major financial institutions from around the globe.  The Forum helps consumers make better financial decisions by fostering collaborative research efforts between academics and industry leaders.

Benartzi’s most significant research contribution is the development of Save More Tomorrow™ (SMarT), a behavioral prescription designed to help employees increase their savings rates gradually over time.

More profile about the speaker
Shlomo Benartzi | Speaker | TED.com
TEDSalon NY2011

Shlomo Benartzi: Saving for tomorrow, tomorrow

Filmed:
1,754,678 views

It's easy to imagine saving money next week, but how about right now? Generally, we want to spend it. Economist Shlomo Benartzi says this is one of the biggest obstacles to saving enough for retirement, and asks: How do we turn this behavioral challenge into a behavioral solution?
- Economist
Shlomo Benartzi uses behavioral economics to study how and why we plan well for the future (or fail to), and uses that to develop new programs to encourage saving for retirement. Full bio

Double-click the English transcript below to play the video.

00:15
I'm going to talk today about saving more,
0
0
3000
00:18
but not today, tomorrow.
1
3000
3000
00:21
I'm going to talk about Save More Tomorrow.
2
6000
2000
00:23
It's a program that Richard Thaler
3
8000
2000
00:25
from the University of Chicago and I
4
10000
2000
00:27
devised maybe 15 years ago.
5
12000
3000
00:30
The program, in a sense,
6
15000
2000
00:32
is an example of behavioral finance
7
17000
2000
00:34
on steroids --
8
19000
2000
00:36
how we could really use behavioral finance.
9
21000
3000
00:39
Now you might ask, what is behavioral finance?
10
24000
3000
00:42
So let's think about how we manage our money.
11
27000
3000
00:45
Let's start with mortgages.
12
30000
3000
00:48
It's kind of a recent topic,
13
33000
2000
00:50
at least in the U.S.
14
35000
2000
00:52
A lot of people buy
15
37000
2000
00:54
the biggest house they can afford,
16
39000
3000
00:57
and actually slightly bigger than that.
17
42000
3000
01:00
And then they foreclose.
18
45000
3000
01:03
And then they blame the banks
19
48000
2000
01:05
for being the bad guys who gave them the mortgages.
20
50000
3000
01:08
Let's also think about
21
53000
2000
01:10
how we manage risks --
22
55000
2000
01:12
for example, investing in the stock market.
23
57000
2000
01:14
Two years ago, three years ago, about four years ago,
24
59000
3000
01:17
markets did well.
25
62000
2000
01:19
We were risk takers, of course.
26
64000
3000
01:22
Then market stocks seize
27
67000
2000
01:24
and we're like, "Wow.
28
69000
2000
01:26
These losses, they feel, emotionally,
29
71000
3000
01:29
they feel very different
30
74000
3000
01:32
from what we actually thought about it
31
77000
3000
01:35
when markets were going up."
32
80000
2000
01:37
So we're probably not doing a great job
33
82000
3000
01:40
when it comes to risk taking.
34
85000
2000
01:42
How many of you have iPhones?
35
87000
3000
01:45
Anyone? Wonderful.
36
90000
3000
01:48
I would bet many more of you
37
93000
3000
01:51
insure your iPhone --
38
96000
3000
01:54
you're implicitly buying insurance by having an extended warranty.
39
99000
3000
01:57
What if you lose your iPhone?
40
102000
2000
01:59
What if you do this?
41
104000
2000
02:01
How many of you have kids?
42
106000
2000
02:03
Anyone?
43
108000
2000
02:05
Keep your hands up
44
110000
2000
02:07
if you have sufficient life insurance.
45
112000
3000
02:10
I see a lot of hands coming down.
46
115000
2000
02:12
I would predict,
47
117000
2000
02:14
if you're a representative sample,
48
119000
2000
02:16
that many more of you
49
121000
2000
02:18
insure your iPhones than your lives,
50
123000
3000
02:21
even when you have kids.
51
126000
2000
02:23
We're not doing that well when it comes to insurance.
52
128000
3000
02:26
The average American household
53
131000
4000
02:30
spends 1,000 dollars a year
54
135000
3000
02:33
on lotteries.
55
138000
2000
02:35
And I know it sounds crazy.
56
140000
3000
02:38
How many of you spend a thousand dollars a year on lotteries?
57
143000
3000
02:41
No one.
58
146000
2000
02:43
So that tells us that the people not in this room
59
148000
3000
02:46
are spending more than a thousand
60
151000
2000
02:48
to get the average to a thousand.
61
153000
3000
02:51
Low-income people
62
156000
2000
02:53
spend a lot more than a thousand on lotteries.
63
158000
4000
02:57
So where does it take us?
64
162000
2000
02:59
We're not doing a great job managing money.
65
164000
3000
03:02
Behavioral finance is really a combination
66
167000
3000
03:05
of psychology and economics,
67
170000
2000
03:07
trying to understand
68
172000
2000
03:09
the money mistakes people make.
69
174000
2000
03:11
And I can keep standing here
70
176000
2000
03:13
for the 12 minutes and 53 seconds that I have left
71
178000
4000
03:17
and make fun of all sorts of ways
72
182000
2000
03:19
we manage money,
73
184000
2000
03:21
and at the end you're going to ask, "How can we help people?"
74
186000
3000
03:24
And that's what I really want to focus on today.
75
189000
3000
03:27
How do we take an understanding
76
192000
2000
03:29
of the money mistakes people make,
77
194000
3000
03:32
and then turning the behavioral challenges
78
197000
3000
03:35
into behavioral solutions?
79
200000
2000
03:37
And what I'm going to talk about today
80
202000
2000
03:39
is Save More Tomorrow.
81
204000
2000
03:41
I want to address the issue
82
206000
2000
03:43
of savings.
83
208000
2000
03:45
We have on the screen
84
210000
2000
03:47
a representative sample
85
212000
2000
03:49
of 100 Americans.
86
214000
2000
03:51
And we're going to look at their saving behavior.
87
216000
3000
03:54
First thing to notice is,
88
219000
2000
03:56
half of them
89
221000
2000
03:58
do not even have access
90
223000
2000
04:00
to a 401(k) plan.
91
225000
2000
04:02
They cannot make savings easy.
92
227000
3000
04:05
They cannot have money go away from their paycheck
93
230000
3000
04:08
into a 401(k) plan
94
233000
2000
04:10
before they see it,
95
235000
2000
04:12
before they can touch it.
96
237000
2000
04:14
What about the remaining half of the people?
97
239000
3000
04:17
Some of them elect not to save.
98
242000
3000
04:20
They're just too lazy.
99
245000
2000
04:22
They never get around to logging into a complicated website
100
247000
3000
04:25
and doing 17 clicks to join the 401(k) plan.
101
250000
3000
04:28
And then they have to decide how they're going to invest
102
253000
2000
04:30
in their 52 choices,
103
255000
2000
04:32
and they never heard about what is a money market fund.
104
257000
4000
04:36
And they get overwhelmed and the just don't join.
105
261000
2000
04:38
How many people end up saving to a 401(k) plan?
106
263000
5000
04:43
One third of Americans.
107
268000
3000
04:46
Two thirds are not saving now.
108
271000
2000
04:48
Are they saving enough?
109
273000
2000
04:50
Take out those
110
275000
2000
04:52
who say they save too little.
111
277000
2000
04:54
One out of 10
112
279000
2000
04:56
are saving enough.
113
281000
3000
04:59
Nine out of 10
114
284000
2000
05:01
either cannot save through their 401(k) plan,
115
286000
3000
05:04
decide not to save -- or don't decide --
116
289000
3000
05:07
or save too little.
117
292000
3000
05:10
We think we have a problem
118
295000
2000
05:12
of people saving too much.
119
297000
2000
05:14
Let's look at that.
120
299000
2000
05:16
We have one person --
121
301000
2000
05:18
well, actually we're going to slice him in half
122
303000
3000
05:21
because it's less than one percent.
123
306000
3000
05:24
Roughly half a percent of Americans
124
309000
3000
05:27
feel that they save too much.
125
312000
5000
05:32
What are we going to do about it?
126
317000
2000
05:34
That's what I really want to focus on.
127
319000
2000
05:36
We have to understand
128
321000
2000
05:38
why people are not saving,
129
323000
2000
05:40
and then we can hopefully flip
130
325000
2000
05:42
the behavioral challenges
131
327000
2000
05:44
into behavioral solutions,
132
329000
2000
05:46
and then see how powerful it might be.
133
331000
3000
05:49
So let me divert for a second
134
334000
2000
05:51
as we're going to identify the problems,
135
336000
2000
05:53
the challenges, the behavioral challenges,
136
338000
3000
05:56
that prevent people from saving.
137
341000
2000
05:58
I'm going to divert and talk about bananas and chocolate.
138
343000
4000
06:02
Suppose we had another wonderful TED event next week.
139
347000
3000
06:05
And during the break
140
350000
2000
06:07
there would be a snack
141
352000
2000
06:09
and you could choose bananas or chocolate.
142
354000
2000
06:11
How many of you think you would like to have bananas
143
356000
3000
06:14
during this hypothetical TED event next week?
144
359000
2000
06:16
Who would go for bananas?
145
361000
2000
06:18
Wonderful.
146
363000
2000
06:20
I predict scientifically
147
365000
2000
06:22
74 percent of you will go for bananas.
148
367000
3000
06:25
Well that's at least what one wonderful study predicted.
149
370000
4000
06:30
And then count down the days
150
375000
3000
06:33
and see what people ended up eating.
151
378000
4000
06:38
The same people that imagined themselves
152
383000
3000
06:41
eating the bananas
153
386000
2000
06:43
ended up eating chocolates
154
388000
2000
06:45
a week later.
155
390000
2000
06:47
Self-control
156
392000
2000
06:49
is not a problem in the future.
157
394000
3000
06:52
It's only a problem now
158
397000
2000
06:54
when the chocolate is next to us.
159
399000
4000
06:58
What does it have to do with time and savings,
160
403000
3000
07:01
this issue of immediate gratification?
161
406000
3000
07:04
Or as some economists call it, present bias.
162
409000
4000
07:08
We think about saving. We know we should be saving.
163
413000
2000
07:10
We know we'll do it next year, but today let us go and spend.
164
415000
3000
07:13
Christmas is coming,
165
418000
2000
07:15
we might as well buy a lot of gifts for everyone we know.
166
420000
3000
07:18
So this issue of present bias
167
423000
4000
07:22
causes us to think about saving,
168
427000
2000
07:24
but end up spending.
169
429000
2000
07:26
Let me now talk
170
431000
2000
07:28
about another behavioral obstacle to saving
171
433000
2000
07:30
having to do with inertia.
172
435000
2000
07:32
But again, a little diversion
173
437000
2000
07:34
to the topic of organ donation.
174
439000
3000
07:37
Wonderful study comparing different countries.
175
442000
3000
07:40
We're going to look at two similar countries,
176
445000
3000
07:43
Germany and Austria.
177
448000
3000
07:46
And in Germany,
178
451000
2000
07:48
if you would like to donate your organs --
179
453000
2000
07:50
God forbid something really bad
180
455000
2000
07:52
happens to you --
181
457000
2000
07:54
when you get your driving license or an I.D.,
182
459000
3000
07:57
you check the box saying,
183
462000
2000
07:59
"I would like to donate my organs."
184
464000
2000
08:01
Not many people like checking boxes.
185
466000
2000
08:03
It takes effort. You need to think.
186
468000
2000
08:05
Twelve percent do.
187
470000
3000
08:08
Austria, a neighboring country,
188
473000
3000
08:11
slightly similar, slightly different.
189
476000
2000
08:13
What's the difference?
190
478000
2000
08:15
Well, you still have choice.
191
480000
2000
08:17
You will decide
192
482000
2000
08:19
whether you want to donate your organs or not.
193
484000
3000
08:22
But when you get your driving license,
194
487000
2000
08:24
you check the box
195
489000
2000
08:26
if you do not want to donate your organ.
196
491000
4000
08:30
Nobody checks boxes.
197
495000
2000
08:32
That's kind of too much effort.
198
497000
2000
08:34
One percent check the box. The rest do nothing.
199
499000
3000
08:37
Doing nothing is very common.
200
502000
2000
08:39
Not many people check boxes.
201
504000
3000
08:42
What are the implications
202
507000
2000
08:44
to saving lives
203
509000
2000
08:46
and having organs available?
204
511000
3000
08:49
In Germany, 12 percent check the box.
205
514000
2000
08:51
Twelve percent are organ donors.
206
516000
3000
08:54
Huge shortage of organs,
207
519000
2000
08:56
God forbid, if you need one.
208
521000
2000
08:58
In Austria, again, nobody checks the box.
209
523000
3000
09:01
Therefore, 99 percent of people
210
526000
3000
09:04
are organ donors.
211
529000
2000
09:06
Inertia, lack of action.
212
531000
2000
09:08
What is the default setting
213
533000
2000
09:10
if people do nothing,
214
535000
2000
09:12
if they keep procrastinating, if they don't check the boxes?
215
537000
3000
09:15
Very powerful.
216
540000
2000
09:17
We're going to talk
217
542000
2000
09:19
about what happens if people are overwhelmed and scared
218
544000
4000
09:23
to make their 401(k) choices.
219
548000
3000
09:26
Are we going to make them automatically join the plan,
220
551000
3000
09:29
or are they going to be left out?
221
554000
2000
09:31
In too many 401(k) plans,
222
556000
3000
09:34
if people do nothing,
223
559000
2000
09:36
it means they're not saving for retirement,
224
561000
3000
09:39
if they don't check the box.
225
564000
2000
09:41
And checking the box takes effort.
226
566000
3000
09:44
So we've chatted about a couple of behavioral challenges.
227
569000
3000
09:47
One more before we flip the challenges into solutions,
228
572000
3000
09:50
having to do with monkeys and apples.
229
575000
2000
09:52
No, no, no, this is a real study
230
577000
2000
09:54
and it's got a lot to do with behavioral economics.
231
579000
4000
09:58
One group of monkeys gets an apple, they're pretty happy.
232
583000
3000
10:01
The other group gets two apples, one is taken away.
233
586000
2000
10:03
They still have an apple left.
234
588000
2000
10:05
They're really mad.
235
590000
3000
10:08
Why have you taken our apple?
236
593000
3000
10:11
This is the notion of loss aversion.
237
596000
3000
10:14
We hate losing stuff,
238
599000
2000
10:16
even if it doesn't mean a lot of risk.
239
601000
3000
10:19
You would hate to go to the ATM,
240
604000
3000
10:22
take out 100 dollars
241
607000
2000
10:24
and notice that you lost one of those $20 bills.
242
609000
2000
10:26
It's very painful,
243
611000
2000
10:28
even though it doesn't mean anything.
244
613000
2000
10:30
Those 20 dollars might have been a quick lunch.
245
615000
4000
10:34
So this notion of loss aversion
246
619000
4000
10:38
kicks in when it comes to savings too,
247
623000
3000
10:41
because people, mentally
248
626000
2000
10:43
and emotionally and intuitively
249
628000
3000
10:46
frame savings as a loss
250
631000
2000
10:48
because I have to cut my spending.
251
633000
3000
10:51
So we talked about
252
636000
2000
10:53
all sorts of behavioral challenges
253
638000
2000
10:55
having to do with savings eventually.
254
640000
4000
10:59
Whether you think about immediate gratification,
255
644000
3000
11:02
and the chocolates versus bananas,
256
647000
3000
11:05
it's just painful to save now.
257
650000
3000
11:08
It's a lot more fun
258
653000
2000
11:10
to spend now.
259
655000
2000
11:12
We talked about inertia and organ donations
260
657000
3000
11:15
and checking the box.
261
660000
2000
11:17
If people have to check a lot of boxes
262
662000
2000
11:19
to join a 401(k) plan,
263
664000
2000
11:21
they're going to keep procrastinating
264
666000
2000
11:23
and not join.
265
668000
2000
11:25
And last, we talked about loss aversion,
266
670000
2000
11:27
and the monkeys and the apples.
267
672000
2000
11:29
If people frame mentally
268
674000
3000
11:32
saving for retirement as a loss,
269
677000
3000
11:35
they're not going to be saving for retirement.
270
680000
3000
11:38
So we've got these challenges,
271
683000
2000
11:40
and what Richard Thaler and I
272
685000
2000
11:42
were always fascinated by --
273
687000
2000
11:44
take behavioral finance,
274
689000
2000
11:46
make it behavioral finance on steroids
275
691000
2000
11:48
or behavioral finance 2.0
276
693000
2000
11:50
or behavioral finance in action --
277
695000
2000
11:52
flip the challenges into solutions.
278
697000
4000
11:56
And we came up with an embarrassingly simple solution
279
701000
3000
11:59
called Save More, not today, Tomorrow.
280
704000
4000
12:03
How is it going to solve the challenges
281
708000
2000
12:05
we chatted about?
282
710000
2000
12:07
If you think about the problem
283
712000
2000
12:09
of bananas versus chocolates,
284
714000
2000
12:11
we think we're going to eat bananas next week.
285
716000
3000
12:14
We think we're going to save more next year.
286
719000
3000
12:17
Save More Tomorrow
287
722000
3000
12:20
invites employees
288
725000
2000
12:22
to save more maybe next year --
289
727000
2000
12:24
sometime in the future
290
729000
2000
12:26
when we can imagine ourselves
291
731000
2000
12:28
eating bananas,
292
733000
2000
12:30
volunteering more in the community,
293
735000
2000
12:32
exercising more and doing all the right things on the planet.
294
737000
4000
12:36
Now we also talked about checking the box
295
741000
3000
12:39
and the difficulty of taking action.
296
744000
3000
12:42
Save More Tomorrow
297
747000
2000
12:44
makes it easy.
298
749000
2000
12:46
It's an autopilot.
299
751000
2000
12:48
Once you tell me you would like to save more in the future,
300
753000
4000
12:52
let's say every January
301
757000
2000
12:54
you're going to be saving more automatically
302
759000
3000
12:57
and it's going to go away from your paycheck to the 401(k) plan
303
762000
3000
13:00
before you see it, before you touch it,
304
765000
2000
13:02
before you get the issue
305
767000
2000
13:04
of immediate gratification.
306
769000
3000
13:07
But what are we going to do about the monkeys
307
772000
3000
13:10
and loss aversion?
308
775000
2000
13:12
Next January comes
309
777000
2000
13:14
and people might feel that if they save more,
310
779000
2000
13:16
they have to spend less, and that's painful.
311
781000
3000
13:20
Well, maybe it shouldn't be just January.
312
785000
2000
13:22
Maybe we should make people save more
313
787000
3000
13:25
when they make more money.
314
790000
3000
13:28
That way, when they make more money, when they get a pay raise,
315
793000
3000
13:31
they don't have to cut their spending.
316
796000
4000
13:35
They take a little bit
317
800000
2000
13:37
of the increase in the paycheck home
318
802000
2000
13:39
and spend more --
319
804000
2000
13:41
take a little bit of the increase
320
806000
2000
13:43
and put it in a 401(k) plan.
321
808000
2000
13:45
So that is the program,
322
810000
2000
13:47
embarrassingly simple,
323
812000
2000
13:49
but as we're going to see,
324
814000
2000
13:51
extremely powerful.
325
816000
2000
13:53
We first implemented it,
326
818000
2000
13:55
Richard Thaler and I,
327
820000
2000
13:57
back in 1998.
328
822000
3000
14:00
Mid-sized company in the Midwest,
329
825000
3000
14:03
blue collar employees
330
828000
2000
14:05
struggling to pay their bills
331
830000
2000
14:07
repeatedly told us
332
832000
2000
14:09
they cannot save more right away.
333
834000
3000
14:12
Saving more today is not an option.
334
837000
3000
14:15
We invited them to save
335
840000
2000
14:17
three percentage points more
336
842000
3000
14:20
every time they get a pay raise.
337
845000
3000
14:23
And here are the results.
338
848000
3000
14:26
We're seeing here a three and a half-year period,
339
851000
2000
14:28
four pay raises,
340
853000
2000
14:30
people who were struggling to save,
341
855000
2000
14:32
were saving three percent of their paycheck,
342
857000
2000
14:34
three and a half years later
343
859000
2000
14:36
saving almost four times as much,
344
861000
3000
14:39
almost 14 percent.
345
864000
3000
14:42
And there's shoes and bicycles
346
867000
2000
14:44
and things on this chart
347
869000
2000
14:46
because I don't want to just throw numbers
348
871000
2000
14:48
in a vacuum.
349
873000
2000
14:50
I want, really, to think about the fact
350
875000
3000
14:53
that saving four times more
351
878000
2000
14:55
is a huge difference
352
880000
2000
14:57
in terms of the lifestyle
353
882000
2000
14:59
that people will be able to afford.
354
884000
2000
15:01
It's real.
355
886000
2000
15:03
It's not just numbers on a piece of paper.
356
888000
3000
15:06
Whereas with saving three percent,
357
891000
2000
15:08
people might have to add nice sneakers
358
893000
2000
15:10
so they can walk,
359
895000
2000
15:12
because they won't be able to afford anything else,
360
897000
4000
15:16
when they save 14 percent
361
901000
2000
15:18
they might be able to maybe have nice dress shoes
362
903000
3000
15:21
to walk to the car to drive.
363
906000
3000
15:24
This is a real difference.
364
909000
2000
15:26
By now, about 60 percent of the large companies
365
911000
5000
15:31
actually have programs like this in place.
366
916000
3000
15:34
It's been part of the Pension Protection Act.
367
919000
3000
15:37
And needless to say that Thaler and I
368
922000
2000
15:39
have been blessed to be part of this program
369
924000
3000
15:42
and make a difference.
370
927000
2000
15:44
Let me wrap
371
929000
2000
15:46
with two key messages.
372
931000
3000
15:49
One is behavioral finance
373
934000
3000
15:52
is extremely powerful.
374
937000
3000
15:55
This is just one example.
375
940000
3000
15:58
Message two
376
943000
2000
16:00
is there's still a lot to do.
377
945000
2000
16:02
This is really the tip of the iceberg.
378
947000
3000
16:05
If you think about people and mortgages
379
950000
3000
16:08
and buying houses and then not being able to pay for it,
380
953000
3000
16:11
we need to think about that.
381
956000
2000
16:13
If you're thinking about people taking too much risk
382
958000
3000
16:16
and not understanding how much risk they're taking
383
961000
3000
16:19
or taking too little risk,
384
964000
2000
16:21
we need to think about that.
385
966000
2000
16:23
If you think about people spending a thousand dollars a year
386
968000
3000
16:26
on lottery tickets,
387
971000
2000
16:28
we need to think about that.
388
973000
2000
16:30
The average actually,
389
975000
2000
16:32
the record is in Singapore.
390
977000
2000
16:34
The average household
391
979000
2000
16:36
spends $4,000 a year on lottery tickets.
392
981000
3000
16:39
We've got a lot to do,
393
984000
2000
16:41
a lot to solve,
394
986000
2000
16:43
also in the retirement area
395
988000
3000
16:46
when it comes to what people do with their money
396
991000
2000
16:48
after retirement.
397
993000
2000
16:50
One last question:
398
995000
2000
16:52
How many of you feel comfortable
399
997000
3000
16:55
that as you're planning for retirement
400
1000000
2000
16:57
you have a really solid plan
401
1002000
3000
17:00
when you're going to retire,
402
1005000
2000
17:02
when you're going to claim Social Security benefits,
403
1007000
3000
17:05
what lifestyle to expect,
404
1010000
2000
17:07
how much to spend every month
405
1012000
2000
17:09
so you're not going to run out of money?
406
1014000
2000
17:11
How many of you feel you have a solid plan for the future
407
1016000
3000
17:14
when it comes to post-retirement decisions.
408
1019000
4000
17:19
One, two, three, four.
409
1024000
3000
17:22
Less than three percent
410
1027000
2000
17:24
of a very sophisticated audience.
411
1029000
2000
17:26
Behavioral finance has a long way.
412
1031000
3000
17:29
There's a lot of opportunities
413
1034000
2000
17:31
to make it powerful again and again and again.
414
1036000
4000
17:35
Thank you.
415
1040000
2000
17:37
(Applause)
416
1042000
2000

▲Back to top

ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Shlomo Benartzi - Economist
Shlomo Benartzi uses behavioral economics to study how and why we plan well for the future (or fail to), and uses that to develop new programs to encourage saving for retirement.

Why you should listen

Shlomo Benartzi studies behavioral finance with a special interest in personal finance. He is co-founder of the Behavioral Finance Forum (www.behavioralfinanceforum.com), a collective of 40 prominent academics and 40 major financial institutions from around the globe.  The Forum helps consumers make better financial decisions by fostering collaborative research efforts between academics and industry leaders.

Benartzi’s most significant research contribution is the development of Save More Tomorrow™ (SMarT), a behavioral prescription designed to help employees increase their savings rates gradually over time.

More profile about the speaker
Shlomo Benartzi | Speaker | TED.com

Data provided by TED.

This site was created in May 2015 and the last update was on January 12, 2020. It will no longer be updated.

We are currently creating a new site called "eng.lish.video" and would be grateful if you could access it.

If you have any questions or suggestions, please feel free to write comments in your language on the contact form.

Privacy Policy

Developer's Blog

Buy Me A Coffee