ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Daniel Kahneman - Behavioral economics founder
Widely regarded as the world's most influential living psychologist, Daniel Kahneman won the Nobel in Economics for his pioneering work in behavioral economics -- exploring the irrational ways we make decisions about risk.

Why you should listen

Daniel Kahneman is an eminence grise for the Freakonomics crowd. In the mid-1970s, with his collaborator Amos Tversky, he was among the first academics to pick apart exactly why we make "wrong" decisions. In their 1979 paper on prospect theory, Kahneman and Tversky examined a simple problem of economic risk. And rather than stating the optimal, rational answer, as an economist of the time might have, they quantified how most real people, consistently, make a less-rational choice. Their work treated economics not as a perfect or self-correcting machine, but as a system prey to quirks of human perception. The field of behavioral economics was born.

Kahneman was awarded the Nobel Memorial prize in 2002 for his work with Tversky, who died before the award was bestowed. In a lovely passage in his Nobel biography, Kahneman looks back on his deep collaboration with Tversky and calls for a new form of academic cooperation, marked not by turf battles but by "adversarial collaboration," a good-faith effort by unlike minds to conduct joint research, critiquing each other in the service of an ideal of truth to which both can contribute.

More profile about the speaker
Daniel Kahneman | Speaker | TED.com
TED2010

Daniel Kahneman: The riddle of experience vs. memory

丹尼爾‧卡內曼(Daniel Kahneman):經驗與記憶之謎

Filmed:
6,094,013 views

身為諾貝爾獎得主及行為金融學創始者丹尼爾‧卡內曼用放假和結腸鏡檢查的例子向我們展示「經驗的我」和「記憶的我」是以不同的方式感受快樂。這項新創見涵括了經濟、政治和我們的對自己的自我意識,意義深遠。
- Behavioral economics founder
Widely regarded as the world's most influential living psychologist, Daniel Kahneman won the Nobel in Economics for his pioneering work in behavioral economics -- exploring the irrational ways we make decisions about risk. Full bio

Double-click the English transcript below to play the video.

00:15
Everybody每個人 talks會談 about happiness幸福 these days.
0
0
3000
最近大家都在談論快樂
00:18
I had somebody count計數 the number of books圖書
1
3000
3000
我請人數了有多少本書
00:21
with "happiness幸福" in the title標題 published發表 in the last five years年份
2
6000
3000
在近五年來出版書名有提到"快樂"
00:24
and they gave up after about 40, and there were many許多 more.
3
9000
5000
他們數到大約超過40本,並且還有更多
00:29
There is a huge巨大 wave of interest利益 in happiness幸福,
4
14000
3000
越來越多人對快樂的議題感到有興趣
00:32
among其中 researchers研究人員.
5
17000
2000
展開研究
00:34
There is a lot of happiness幸福 coaching教練.
6
19000
2000
其中更多訓練教導人如何變快樂
00:36
Everybody每個人 would like to make people happier幸福.
7
21000
2000
人人都想讓人更快樂
00:38
But in spite儘管 of all this flood洪水 of work,
8
23000
4000
雖這類書籍多如牛毛
00:42
there are several一些 cognitive認知 traps陷阱
9
27000
2000
仍有些認知上的陷阱
00:44
that sort分類 of make it almost幾乎 impossible不可能 to think straight直行
10
29000
3000
讓人幾乎難以直接領會
00:47
about happiness幸福.
11
32000
2000
快樂的本質
00:49
And my talk today今天 will be mostly大多 about these cognitive認知 traps陷阱.
12
34000
3000
我今天主要談論的就是這些認知陷阱
00:52
This applies適用 to laypeople外行人 thinking思維 about their own擁有 happiness幸福,
13
37000
3000
它們既會影響一般人對自身快樂的觀感
00:55
and it applies適用 to scholars學者 thinking思維 about happiness幸福,
14
40000
3000
也會影響學者對快樂的判斷
00:58
because it turns out we're just as messed搞砸 up as anybody任何人 else其他 is.
15
43000
4000
因為我們和任何人一樣都會出錯
01:02
The first of these traps陷阱
16
47000
2000
第一個陷阱
01:04
is a reluctance不情願 to admit承認 complexity複雜.
17
49000
3000
是不願去承認快樂並不簡單
01:07
It turns out that the word "happiness幸福"
18
52000
3000
事實證明快樂這個詞
01:10
is just not a useful有用 word anymore,
19
55000
3000
已經再也不實用了
01:13
because we apply應用 it to too many許多 different不同 things.
20
58000
3000
因為我們拿它來詮釋太多事情
01:16
I think there is one particular特定 meaning含義 to which哪一個 we might威力 restrict限制 it,
21
61000
3000
我想我們應該限定它的意思
01:19
but by and large,
22
64000
2000
不過,一般而言
01:21
this is something that we'll have to give up
23
66000
2000
我們得放棄這個想法
01:23
and we'll have to adopt採用 the more complicated複雜 view視圖
24
68000
4000
並用更複雜的觀點來看
01:27
of what well-being福利 is.
25
72000
2000
何謂快樂生活
01:29
The second第二 trap陷阱 is a confusion混亂 between之間 experience經驗 and memory記憶;
26
74000
4000
第二個陷阱是經驗和記憶間的混淆
01:33
basically基本上, it's between之間 being存在 happy快樂 in your life,
27
78000
3000
基本上這是在生活中體會快樂
01:36
and being存在 happy快樂 about your life
28
81000
2000
和覺得生活很快樂
01:38
or happy快樂 with your life.
29
83000
2000
樂觀生活之間的差別
01:40
And those are two very different不同 concepts概念,
30
85000
2000
這兩者意義相差甚遠
01:42
and they're both lumped集總 in the notion概念 of happiness幸福.
31
87000
3000
論及快樂常混為一談
01:45
And the third第三 is the focusing調焦 illusion錯覺,
32
90000
3000
第三點是大事幻覺論
01:48
and it's the unfortunate不幸的 fact事實 that we can't think about any circumstance環境
33
93000
3000
不幸,當我們想到一些
01:51
that affects影響 well-being福利
34
96000
2000
關於快樂生活的情境
01:53
without distorting扭曲 its importance重要性.
35
98000
2000
勢必會覺得它特別重要
01:55
I mean, this is a real真實 cognitive認知 trap陷阱.
36
100000
3000
這是真正的認知陷阱
01:58
There's just no way of getting得到 it right.
37
103000
3000
避無可避
02:01
Now, I'd like to start開始 with an example
38
106000
2000
現在,我想以一個例子開頭
02:03
of somebody who had a question-and-answer問題和答案 session會議
39
108000
5000
有個人上過我的課後
02:08
after one of my lectures講座 reported報導 a story故事,
40
113000
4000
在Q&A回應了一則故事
02:12
and that was a story故事 --
41
117000
1000
這個故事是這樣的
02:13
He said he'd他會 been listening to a symphony交響樂,
42
118000
3000
他說他有次在聽交響樂時
02:16
and it was absolutely絕對 glorious輝煌 music音樂
43
121000
3000
覺得音樂真是動聽極了
02:19
and at the very end結束 of the recording記錄,
44
124000
3000
但在演奏快結束時
02:22
there was a dreadful可怕 screeching尖叫 sound聲音.
45
127000
2000
卻冒出了可怕刺耳的聲音
02:24
And then he added添加, really quite相當 emotionally感情上,
46
129000
2000
接著他生氣地表示
02:26
it ruined毀了 the whole整個 experience經驗.
47
131000
4000
這毀了整個愉快的經驗
02:30
But it hadn't有沒有.
48
135000
2000
事實並非如此
02:32
What it had ruined毀了 were the memories回憶 of the experience經驗.
49
137000
3000
毀了的是對於這段經驗的記憶
02:35
He had had the experience經驗.
50
140000
2000
他經歷了這段經驗
02:37
He had had 20 minutes分鐘 of glorious輝煌 music音樂.
51
142000
2000
經歷了20分鐘的聽覺饗宴
02:39
They counted for nothing
52
144000
2000
現在都不算數了
02:41
because he was left with a memory記憶;
53
146000
3000
因為他只剩下記憶
02:44
the memory記憶 was ruined毀了,
54
149000
2000
有污點的記憶
02:46
and the memory記憶 was all that he had gotten得到 to keep.
55
151000
3000
他就只剩下這段記憶
02:49
What this is telling告訴 us, really,
56
154000
3000
這告訴我們
02:52
is that we might威力 be thinking思維 of ourselves我們自己 and of other people
57
157000
2000
我們在思考自己和別人時
02:54
in terms條款 of two selves自我.
58
159000
2000
用了兩種我
02:56
There is an experiencing經歷 self,
59
161000
3000
經驗的我
02:59
who lives生活 in the present當下
60
164000
2000
活在當下
03:01
and knows知道 the present當下,
61
166000
2000
感受當下
03:03
is capable of re-living重新生活 the past過去,
62
168000
2000
也能感受過去的經驗
03:05
but basically基本上 it has only the present當下.
63
170000
3000
但基本上他只屬於當下
03:08
It's the experiencing經歷 self that the doctor醫生 approaches方法 --
64
173000
3000
當醫生要接觸的是經驗的我
03:11
you know, when the doctor醫生 asks,
65
176000
1000
他會問
03:12
"Does it hurt傷害 now when I touch觸摸 you here?"
66
177000
4000
"我碰你這裡會痛嗎?"
03:16
And then there is a remembering記憶 self,
67
181000
3000
另一個是記憶的我
03:19
and the remembering記憶 self is the one that keeps保持 score得分了,
68
184000
4000
負責記錄生活
03:23
and maintains維持 the story故事 of our life,
69
188000
2000
抒寫生活故事
03:25
and it's the one that the doctor醫生 approaches方法
70
190000
3000
醫生要找他時
03:28
in asking the question,
71
193000
2000
會這麼問
03:30
"How have you been feeling感覺 lately最近?"
72
195000
3000
"最近感覺如何?"
03:33
or "How was your trip to Albania阿爾巴尼亞?" or something like that.
73
198000
3000
或"去阿爾巴尼亞好玩嗎",類似的問題
03:36
Those are two very different不同 entities實體,
74
201000
3000
經驗的我和記憶的我
03:39
the experiencing經歷 self and the remembering記憶 self,
75
204000
3000
兩者十分不同
03:42
and getting得到 confused困惑 between之間 them is part部分 of the mess食堂
76
207000
4000
我們之所以不懂快樂
03:46
about the notion概念 of happiness幸福.
77
211000
3000
兩者的混淆是部分原因
03:49
Now, the remembering記憶 self
78
214000
3000
記憶的我
03:52
is a storyteller說故事的人.
79
217000
3000
負責說故事
03:55
And that really starts啟動 with a basic基本 response響應 of our memories回憶 --
80
220000
4000
故事從記憶中直接擷取
03:59
it starts啟動 immediately立即.
81
224000
2000
即時上傳
04:01
We don't only tell stories故事 when we set out to tell stories故事.
82
226000
3000
我們講故事的時候並不是我們在講
04:04
Our memory記憶 tells告訴 us stories故事,
83
229000
3000
是我們的記憶在說故事
04:07
that is, what we get to keep from our experiences經驗
84
232000
2000
我們從經驗中儲存下來的
04:09
is a story故事.
85
234000
2000
是故事
04:11
And let me begin開始 with one example.
86
236000
5000
讓我用一個例子開頭
04:16
This is an old study研究.
87
241000
2000
以前有個研究
04:18
Those are actual實際 patients耐心 undergoing經歷 a painful痛苦 procedure程序.
88
243000
3000
由真正的病人接受痛苦的治療
04:21
I won't慣於 go into detail詳情. It's no longer painful痛苦 these days,
89
246000
3000
細節不多說,現今的療法已不再難受
04:24
but it was painful痛苦 when this study研究 was run in the 1990s.
90
249000
4000
但在實驗進行的90年代,治療令人痛不欲生
04:28
They were asked to report報告 on their pain疼痛 every一切 60 seconds.
91
253000
3000
病人必須每分鐘報告痛苦指數
04:31
Here are two patients耐心,
92
256000
3000
這邊有兩個病患
04:34
those are their recordings錄音.
93
259000
2000
還有他們的紀錄
04:36
And you are asked, "Who of them suffered遭遇 more?"
94
261000
3000
我問你,"誰受苦多一點?"
04:39
And it's a very easy簡單 question.
95
264000
2000
這問題很簡單
04:41
Clearly明確地, Patient患者 B suffered遭遇 more --
96
266000
2000
顯然,是病人B
04:43
his colonoscopy結腸鏡檢查 was longer,
97
268000
2000
他的結腸鏡檢查時間較長
04:45
and every一切 minute分鐘 of pain疼痛 that Patient患者 A had,
98
270000
3000
病人A每分鐘感覺的疼痛
04:48
Patient患者 B had, and more.
99
273000
3000
病人B也感覺到了,而且更久
04:51
But now there is another另一個 question:
100
276000
3000
但現在有另一個問題
04:54
"How much did these patients耐心 think they suffered遭遇?"
101
279000
3000
"誰感覺比較痛?"
04:57
And here is a surprise.
102
282000
2000
這裡有個小意外
04:59
The surprise is that Patient患者 A
103
284000
2000
意外的是病人A
05:01
had a much worse更差 memory記憶 of the colonoscopy結腸鏡檢查
104
286000
3000
對於結腸鏡檢查的記憶比病人B
05:04
than Patient患者 B.
105
289000
2000
還糟糕
05:06
The stories故事 of the colonoscopies結腸鏡檢查 were different不同,
106
291000
3000
兩段結腸鏡檢查的故事不同
05:09
and because a very critical危急 part部分 of the story故事 is how it ends結束.
107
294000
6000
重點在於故事的結尾
05:15
And neither也不 of these stories故事 is very inspiring鼓舞人心 or great --
108
300000
3000
兩個故事都不怎麼啟發人心
05:18
but one of them is this distinct不同 ... (Laughter笑聲)
109
303000
4000
不過其中之一
05:22
but one of them is distinctly歷歷 worse更差 than the other.
110
307000
3000
顯然感受比另一個還差
05:25
And the one that is worse更差
111
310000
2000
感覺較糟的這一個
05:27
is the one where pain疼痛 was at its peak at the very end結束;
112
312000
3000
結尾時處於疼痛高峰
05:30
it's a bad story故事.
113
315000
2000
這不是個好故事
05:32
How do we know that?
114
317000
2000
我們怎知道的?
05:34
Because we asked these people after their colonoscopy結腸鏡檢查,
115
319000
3000
因為我們在檢查結束後問他們
05:37
and much later後來, too,
116
322000
1000
稍晚之後問說
05:38
"How bad was the whole整個 thing, in total?"
117
323000
2000
"整體而言,感覺多糟?"
05:40
And it was much worse更差 for A than for B, in memory記憶.
118
325000
4000
而A的記憶感覺比B糟糕許多
05:44
Now this is a direct直接 conflict衝突
119
329000
2000
顯然經驗的我和記憶的我之間
05:46
between之間 the experiencing經歷 self and the remembering記憶 self.
120
331000
3000
有了直接衝突
05:49
From the point of view視圖 of the experiencing經歷 self,
121
334000
3000
就經驗的我來看
05:52
clearly明確地, B had a worse更差 time.
122
337000
2000
顯然B比較難受
05:54
Now, what you could do with Patient患者 A,
123
339000
3000
那病人A的情況該怎麼解釋
05:57
and we actually其實 ran clinical臨床 experiments實驗,
124
342000
3000
我們實際做了臨床實驗
06:00
and it has been doneDONE, and it does work --
125
345000
2000
實驗完成了,確實有用
06:02
you could actually其實 extend延伸 the colonoscopy結腸鏡檢查 of Patient患者 A
126
347000
5000
病人A的檢查結果能得到解釋
06:07
by just keeping保持 the tube in without jiggling輕搖 it too much.
127
352000
3000
在於持續插入導管時不晃動得太厲害
06:10
That will cause原因 the patient患者
128
355000
3000
雖然病人還是會痛
06:13
to suffer遭受, but just a little
129
358000
3000
但只有一點點
06:16
and much less than before.
130
361000
2000
比先前好受許多
06:18
And if you do that for a couple一對 of minutes分鐘,
131
363000
2000
繼續這樣做,幾分鐘下來
06:20
you have made製作 the experiencing經歷 self
132
365000
2000
病人A的經驗的我
06:22
of Patient患者 A worse更差 off,
133
367000
2000
感覺每況愈下
06:24
and you have the remembering記憶 self of Patient患者 A
134
369000
3000
而病人A記憶的我
06:27
a lot better off,
135
372000
2000
感覺好多了
06:29
because now you have endowed天賦 Patient患者 A
136
374000
2000
因為你給了病人A
06:31
with a better story故事
137
376000
2000
一個好一點的故事
06:33
about his experience經驗.
138
378000
3000
記憶他的經驗
06:36
What defines定義 a story故事?
139
381000
3000
故事好壞由什麼決定?
06:39
And that is true真正 of the stories故事
140
384000
2000
記憶告訴我們的故事
06:41
that memory記憶 delivers提供 for us,
141
386000
2000
是真實的
06:43
and it's also true真正 of the stories故事 that we make up.
142
388000
3000
我們講的故事也是真的
06:46
What defines定義 a story故事 are changes變化,
143
391000
4000
故事好壞取決於變化
06:50
significant重大 moments瞬間 and endings結局.
144
395000
3000
重要時刻和結尾的變化
06:53
Endings結局 are very, very important重要
145
398000
2000
結尾非常重要
06:55
and, in this case案件, the ending結尾 dominated佔主導地位.
146
400000
4000
上面的例子是結尾由主導
06:59
Now, the experiencing經歷 self
147
404000
2000
經驗的我
07:01
lives生活 its life continuously一直.
148
406000
3000
延續這個生活經驗
07:04
It has moments瞬間 of experience經驗, one after the other.
149
409000
3000
擁有各種時刻的經驗,一個接一個
07:07
And you can ask: What happens發生 to these moments瞬間?
150
412000
3000
你會問"這些時刻怎麼了?"
07:10
And the answer回答 is really straightforward直截了當:
151
415000
2000
答案很簡單
07:12
They are lost丟失 forever永遠.
152
417000
2000
他們永遠消失了
07:14
I mean, most of the moments瞬間 of our life --
153
419000
2000
我們生活中大多數時刻
07:16
and I calculated計算, you know, the psychological心理 present當下
154
421000
3000
我算了算,以心理學上的現在而言
07:19
is said to be about three seconds long;
155
424000
2000
據說長達三秒
07:21
that means手段 that, you know,
156
426000
2000
這表示
07:23
in a life there are about 600 million百萬 of them;
157
428000
2000
一個人一生中有大約6億個時刻
07:25
in a month, there are about 600,000 --
158
430000
3000
一個月有60萬個時刻
07:28
most of them don't leave離開 a trace跟踪.
159
433000
4000
它們大多數不留痕跡
07:32
Most of them are completely全然 ignored忽視
160
437000
2000
大多數被全然忽視了
07:34
by the remembering記憶 self.
161
439000
2000
被記憶的我忽視
07:36
And yet然而, somehow不知何故 you get the sense
162
441000
2000
但是現在你發現
07:38
that they should count計數,
163
443000
2000
他們應該也算數
07:40
that what happens發生 during these moments瞬間 of experience經驗
164
445000
3000
因為我們經驗的每分每秒發生的一切
07:43
is our life.
165
448000
2000
組成了我們的人生
07:45
It's the finite有限 resource資源 that we're spending開支
166
450000
2000
他們是我們活在地球上
07:47
while we're on this earth地球.
167
452000
2000
就會消耗的有限資源
07:49
And how to spend it
168
454000
2000
該如何使用
07:51
would seem似乎 to be relevant相應,
169
456000
2000
似乎很重要
07:53
but that is not the story故事
170
458000
2000
但這不是故事
07:55
that the remembering記憶 self keeps保持 for us.
171
460000
2000
也不是記憶的我會留給我們的
07:57
So we have the remembering記憶 self
172
462000
2000
我們有記憶的我
07:59
and the experiencing經歷 self,
173
464000
2000
和經驗的我
08:01
and they're really quite相當 distinct不同.
174
466000
2000
兩者很好區別
08:03
The biggest最大 difference區別 between之間 them
175
468000
2000
其中最大的不同
08:05
is in the handling處理 of time.
176
470000
3000
在於處理時間的方式
08:08
From the point of view視圖 of the experiencing經歷 self,
177
473000
3000
就經驗的我而言
08:11
if you have a vacation假期,
178
476000
2000
如果你有一個假期
08:13
and the second第二 week is just as good as the first,
179
478000
3000
第二週和第一週同等快樂
08:16
then the two-week2週 vacation假期
180
481000
3000
那麼兩週下來
08:19
is twice兩次 as good as the one-week一周 vacation假期.
181
484000
3000
快樂的份量是一週假期的兩倍
08:22
That's not the way it works作品 at all for the remembering記憶 self.
182
487000
3000
記憶的我不是這樣算
08:25
For the remembering記憶 self, a two-week2週 vacation假期
183
490000
2000
對記憶的我來說,兩週假期
08:27
is barely僅僅 better than the one-week一周 vacation假期
184
492000
3000
沒有比一週假期好多少
08:30
because there are no new memories回憶 added添加.
185
495000
2000
因為沒有新的記憶加入
08:32
You have not changed the story故事.
186
497000
3000
你沒有改變故事的劇情
08:35
And in this way,
187
500000
2000
因此
08:37
time is actually其實 the critical危急 variable變量
188
502000
3000
以時間長短
08:40
that distinguishes區分 a remembering記憶 self
189
505000
3000
區分記憶的我和經驗的我
08:43
from an experiencing經歷 self;
190
508000
2000
是不可靠的
08:45
time has very little impact碰撞 on the story故事.
191
510000
3000
時間對故事影響不大
08:49
Now, the remembering記憶 self does more
192
514000
3000
記憶的我所做的
08:52
than remember記得 and tell stories故事.
193
517000
2000
不只記憶和說故事
08:54
It is actually其實 the one that makes品牌 decisions決定
194
519000
4000
他也是真正做決定的人
08:58
because, if you have a patient患者 who has had, say,
195
523000
2000
因為,假如你的病人有選擇權
09:00
two colonoscopies結腸鏡檢查 with two different不同 surgeons外科醫生
196
525000
3000
先由兩個醫生分別檢查一次
09:03
and is deciding決定 which哪一個 of them to choose選擇,
197
528000
3000
然後選擇醫生
09:06
then the one that chooses
198
531000
3000
負責選擇的
09:09
is the one that has the memory記憶 that is less bad,
199
534000
4000
會選擇感覺較好的記憶中
09:13
and that's the surgeon外科醫生 that will be chosen選擇.
200
538000
2000
執行檢查的醫生
09:15
The experiencing經歷 self
201
540000
2000
經驗的我
09:17
has no voice語音 in this choice選擇.
202
542000
3000
做選擇時無從置喙
09:20
We actually其實 don't choose選擇 between之間 experiences經驗,
203
545000
3000
我們事實上不是在兩段經驗中做選擇
09:23
we choose選擇 between之間 memories回憶 of experiences經驗.
204
548000
3000
而是在兩段經驗的記憶中選擇
09:26
And even when we think about the future未來,
205
551000
3000
而且,當我們在想未來的時候
09:29
we don't think of our future未來 normally一般 as experiences經驗.
206
554000
3000
一般我們不會以經驗的形式去思考
09:32
We think of our future未來
207
557000
2000
我們把未來
09:34
as anticipated預期 memories回憶.
208
559000
3000
以預想的記憶形式呈現
09:37
And basically基本上 you can look at this,
209
562000
2000
基本上你可以看這個
09:39
you know, as a tyranny暴政 of the remembering記憶 self,
210
564000
3000
記憶的我是個暴君
09:42
and you can think of the remembering記憶 self
211
567000
2000
你可以把記憶的我想成
09:44
sort分類 of dragging拖延 the experiencing經歷 self
212
569000
2000
有點像是在跩著經驗的我
09:46
through通過 experiences經驗 that
213
571000
2000
透過經驗的我用不著的經驗
09:48
the experiencing經歷 self doesn't need.
214
573000
2000
主宰個人的思考
09:50
I have that sense that
215
575000
2000
我有個想法
09:52
when we go on vacations休假
216
577000
2000
當我們放假時
09:54
this is very frequently經常 the case案件;
217
579000
2000
往往
09:56
that is, we go on vacations休假,
218
581000
2000
之所以放假
09:58
to a very large extent程度,
219
583000
2000
有很大一部分
10:00
in the service服務 of our remembering記憶 self.
220
585000
3000
是為了記憶的我
10:03
And this is a bit hard to justify辯解 I think.
221
588000
3000
我想這有點難辯證
10:06
I mean, how much do we consume消耗 our memories回憶?
222
591000
3000
我們使用了多少記憶
10:09
That is one of the explanations說明
223
594000
2000
可以解釋
10:11
that is given特定 for the dominance霸主地位
224
596000
2000
記憶的我
10:13
of the remembering記憶 self.
225
598000
2000
為何能主導
10:15
And when I think about that, I think about a vacation假期
226
600000
2000
這讓我想起一次假期
10:17
we had in Antarctica南極洲 a few少數 years年份 ago,
227
602000
3000
幾年前在南極度過的假期
10:20
which哪一個 was clearly明確地 the best最好 vacation假期 I've ever had,
228
605000
3000
可說是我最棒的一個假期
10:23
and I think of it relatively相對 often經常,
229
608000
2000
和其他假期相比
10:25
relative相對的 to how much I think of other vacations休假.
230
610000
2000
我比較常想起他
10:27
And I probably大概 have consumed消費
231
612000
4000
這趟三周的旅程
10:31
my memories回憶 of that three-week三個星期 trip, I would say,
232
616000
2000
我大概用掉了
10:33
for about 25 minutes分鐘 in the last four years年份.
233
618000
3000
過去四年來大約25分鐘的記憶量
10:36
Now, if I had ever opened打開 the folder
234
621000
3000
現在,如果我打開資料夾
10:39
with the 600 pictures圖片 in it,
235
624000
3000
裡面有600張照片
10:42
I would have spent花費 another另一個 hour小時.
236
627000
2000
我可能要多花一小時回憶
10:44
Now, that is three weeks,
237
629000
2000
三周的旅程
10:46
and that is at most an hour小時 and a half.
238
631000
2000
最多用一個半小時回憶
10:48
There seems似乎 to be a discrepancy差異.
239
633000
2000
這似乎不成比例
10:50
Now, I may可能 be a bit extreme極端, you know,
240
635000
2000
現在,這讓我有點不滿
10:52
in how little appetite食慾 I have for consuming消費 memories回憶,
241
637000
3000
因為我記得的真是太少了
10:55
but even if you do more of this,
242
640000
3000
不過就算你記得再多
10:58
there is a genuine真正 question:
243
643000
3000
這裡有個實際的問題
11:01
Why do we put so much weight重量 on memory記憶
244
646000
4000
為什麼我們這麼依賴記憶
11:05
relative相對的 to the weight重量 that we put on experiences經驗?
245
650000
3000
比依賴經驗還多
11:08
So I want you to think
246
653000
2000
所以我希望你能思考
11:10
about a thought experiment實驗.
247
655000
3000
一個有關思考的實驗
11:13
Imagine想像 that for your next下一個 vacation假期,
248
658000
2000
想像你的下次假期
11:15
you know that at the end結束 of the vacation假期
249
660000
3000
你知道假期結束後
11:18
all your pictures圖片 will be destroyed銷毀,
250
663000
3000
你的所有照片會被銷毀
11:21
and you'll你會 get an amnesic遺忘 drug藥物
251
666000
2000
你將吞下一顆遺忘藥
11:23
so that you won't慣於 remember記得 anything.
252
668000
2000
所以你什麼都不會記得
11:25
Now, would you choose選擇 the same相同 vacation假期? (Laughter笑聲)
253
670000
4000
這樣,你還會想過同樣的假期嗎?
11:29
And if you would choose選擇 a different不同 vacation假期,
254
674000
5000
如果你選了個不同的假期
11:34
there is a conflict衝突 between之間 your two selves自我,
255
679000
2000
你的兩個我之間將產生衝突
11:36
and you need to think about how to adjudicate裁定 that conflict衝突,
256
681000
3000
你得想辦法裁決這衝突
11:39
and it's actually其實 not at all obvious明顯, because
257
684000
3000
這不怎麼容易
11:42
if you think in terms條款 of time,
258
687000
3000
因為若你考慮到時間
11:45
then you get one answer回答,
259
690000
3000
你會選不同的假期
11:48
and if you think in terms條款 of memories回憶,
260
693000
3000
但若你考量到記憶
11:51
you might威力 get another另一個 answer回答.
261
696000
2000
你可能會選相同的假期
11:53
Why do we pick the vacations休假 we do
262
698000
3000
我們為什麼選此而非彼
11:56
is a problem問題 that confronts面對 us
263
701000
3000
所要面對的問題
11:59
with a choice選擇 between之間 the two selves自我.
264
704000
2000
是在兩個我之間作選擇
12:01
Now, the two selves自我
265
706000
3000
現在,這兩個我
12:04
bring帶來 up two notions概念 of happiness幸福.
266
709000
2000
帶來兩種對快樂的見解
12:06
There are really two concepts概念 of happiness幸福
267
711000
2000
這兩套見解
12:08
that we can apply應用, one per self.
268
713000
3000
分別對應了兩個我
12:11
So you can ask: How happy快樂 is the experiencing經歷 self?
269
716000
5000
因此你會問"經驗的我有多快樂?"
12:16
And then you would ask: How happy快樂 are the moments瞬間
270
721000
2000
接著問,經驗的我
12:18
in the experiencing經歷 self's自我的 life?
271
723000
3000
每一刻有多快樂?
12:21
And they're all -- happiness幸福 for moments瞬間
272
726000
2000
所有這些快樂的時刻
12:23
is a fairly相當 complicated複雜 process處理.
273
728000
2000
組成過程相當複雜
12:25
What are the emotions情緒 that can be measured測量?
274
730000
3000
情感該如何測量?
12:28
And, by the way, now we are capable
275
733000
2000
順帶一提,我們現在
12:30
of getting得到 a pretty漂亮 good idea理念
276
735000
2000
對於經驗的我隨著時間感受快樂
12:32
of the happiness幸福 of the experiencing經歷 self over time.
277
737000
4000
已經有了概念
12:38
If you ask for the happiness幸福 of the remembering記憶 self,
278
743000
3000
而記憶的我所指的快樂
12:41
it's a completely全然 different不同 thing.
279
746000
2000
概念完全不同
12:43
This is not about how happily高高興興 a person lives生活.
280
748000
3000
這不是一個人生活多快樂的問題
12:46
It is about how satisfied滿意 or pleased滿意 the person is
281
751000
3000
而是他對於自己的人生
12:49
when that person thinks about her life.
282
754000
4000
多滿意,多喜歡
12:53
Very different不同 notion概念.
283
758000
2000
見解南轅北轍
12:55
Anyone任何人 who doesn't distinguish區分 those notions概念
284
760000
3000
若不能分辨這兩項見解
12:58
is going to mess食堂 up the study研究 of happiness幸福,
285
763000
2000
就參不透快樂這門學問
13:00
and I belong屬於 to a crowd人群 of students學生們 of well-being福利,
286
765000
3000
我和其他活得好的學生一樣
13:03
who've誰一直 been messing搞亂 up the study研究 of happiness幸福 for a long time
287
768000
4000
一直以來參不透快樂
13:07
in precisely恰恰 this way.
288
772000
2000
就是這個原因
13:09
The distinction分別 between之間 the
289
774000
2000
近年來
13:11
happiness幸福 of the experiencing經歷 self
290
776000
2000
經驗的我的快樂
13:13
and the satisfaction滿意 of the remembering記憶 self
291
778000
3000
和記憶的我的滿足
13:16
has been recognized認可 in recent最近 years年份,
292
781000
2000
之間的差別已能分清
13:18
and there are now efforts努力 to measure測量 the two separately分別.
293
783000
3000
目前正努力要分別測量兩者的程度
13:21
The Gallup蓋洛普 Organization組織 has a world世界 poll輪詢
294
786000
3000
蓋洛普在世界各地做民調
13:24
where more than half a million百萬 people
295
789000
2000
其中超過50萬人
13:26
have been asked questions問題
296
791000
2000
曾被問到一些問題
13:28
about what they think of their life
297
793000
2000
看他們如何看待自己的生活
13:30
and about their experiences經驗,
298
795000
2000
以及他們如何看待經驗
13:32
and there have been other efforts努力 along沿 those lines.
299
797000
3000
這些問題外,也做了不少調查
13:35
So in recent最近 years年份, we have begun開始 to learn學習
300
800000
3000
所以近年來,我們逐漸得知
13:38
about the happiness幸福 of the two selves自我.
301
803000
3000
兩個我個別的快樂
13:41
And the main主要 lesson I think that we have learned學到了
302
806000
3000
而我們主要學到的便是
13:44
is they are really different不同.
303
809000
2000
兩者真的很不一樣
13:46
You can know how satisfied滿意 somebody is with their life,
304
811000
5000
你可以知道一個人對生活是否滿意
13:51
and that really doesn't teach you much
305
816000
2000
但這並不能告訴你
13:53
about how happily高高興興 they're living活的 their life,
306
818000
3000
他們平常活得多快樂
13:56
and vice versa反之亦然.
307
821000
2000
反之亦然
13:58
Just to give you a sense of the correlation相關,
308
823000
2000
只是要讓你對其中的關聯有概念
14:00
the correlation相關 is about .5.
309
825000
2000
關聯約有五點
14:02
What that means手段 is if you met會見 somebody,
310
827000
3000
意思是當你見到某個人
14:05
and you were told, "Oh his father父親 is six feet tall,"
311
830000
4000
並被告知他爸爸六呎高
14:09
how much would you know about his height高度?
312
834000
2000
你能知道他有多高嗎?
14:11
Well, you would know something about his height高度,
313
836000
2000
好吧,你可能有個概念
14:13
but there's a lot of uncertainty不確定.
314
838000
2000
但非常不確定
14:15
You have that much uncertainty不確定.
315
840000
2000
你沒法篤定
14:17
If I tell you that somebody ranked排名 their life eight on a scale規模 of ten,
316
842000
4000
若我說有人10分裡給自己的生活8分
14:21
you have a lot of uncertainty不確定
317
846000
2000
你不能把握
14:23
about how happy快樂 they are
318
848000
2000
他們經驗的我
14:25
with their experiencing經歷 self.
319
850000
2000
有多快樂
14:27
So the correlation相關 is low.
320
852000
2000
所以關聯性很低
14:29
We know something about what controls控制
321
854000
3000
我們知道什麼能夠
14:32
satisfaction滿意 of the happiness幸福 self.
322
857000
2000
讓人的快樂得到滿足
14:34
We know that money is very important重要,
323
859000
2000
我們知道錢很重要
14:36
goals目標 are very important重要.
324
861000
2000
目標非常重要
14:38
We know that happiness幸福 is mainly主要
325
863000
4000
我們知道要滿足快樂
14:42
being存在 satisfied滿意 with people that we like,
326
867000
3000
主要是由我們喜愛的人
14:45
spending開支 time with people that we like.
327
870000
3000
和他們在一起的時間來感受
14:48
There are other pleasures樂趣, but this is dominant優勢.
328
873000
2000
雖然有其他的原因,但這是最主要的
14:50
So if you want to maximize最大化 the happiness幸福 of the two selves自我,
329
875000
3000
所以若你想讓兩個我都超快樂
14:53
you are going to end結束 up
330
878000
2000
你必須拋開過去的方式
14:55
doing very different不同 things.
331
880000
2000
做一些很不同的事情
14:57
The bottom底部 line of what I've said here
332
882000
2000
我說的是,至少
14:59
is that we really should not think of happiness幸福
333
884000
4000
我們不應該把快樂
15:03
as a substitute替代 for well-being福利.
334
888000
2000
當作活得好的代名詞
15:05
It is a completely全然 different不同 notion概念.
335
890000
3000
兩個完全不一樣
15:08
Now, very quickly很快,
336
893000
3000
現在,很快地
15:11
another另一個 reason原因 we cannot不能 think straight直行 about happiness幸福
337
896000
4000
另個我們不能理解快樂本質的原因
15:15
is that we do not attend出席 to the same相同 things
338
900000
7000
在於我們所指不同
15:22
when we think about life, and we actually其實 live生活.
339
907000
3000
怎麼看生活,和實際怎麼過生活不一樣
15:25
So, if you ask the simple簡單 question of how happy快樂 people are in California加州,
340
910000
5000
所以當你問加州的人有多快樂
15:30
you are not going to get to the correct正確 answer回答.
341
915000
3000
你將得不到正確答案
15:33
When you ask that question,
342
918000
2000
因為當你這麼問時
15:35
you think people must必須 be happier幸福 in California加州
343
920000
2000
你認為住加州一定比較快樂
15:37
if, say, you live生活 in Ohio俄亥俄州.
344
922000
2000
剛好,你住在俄亥俄洲
15:39
(Laughter笑聲)
345
924000
2000
(笑聲)
15:41
And what happens發生 is
346
926000
3000
當你在想
15:44
when you think about living活的 in California加州,
347
929000
4000
住在加州有多快樂時
15:48
you are thinking思維 of the contrast對比
348
933000
2000
你會想到
15:50
between之間 California加州 and other places地方,
349
935000
3000
加州和其他地方的差異
15:53
and that contrast對比, say, is in climate氣候.
350
938000
2000
差異,其實是氣候的不同
15:55
Well, it turns out that climate氣候
351
940000
2000
這也證明氣候條件
15:57
is not very important重要 to the experiencing經歷 self
352
942000
3000
對於經驗的我並不很重要
16:00
and it's not even very important重要 to the reflective反光 self
353
945000
3000
事實上對衡量人有多快樂
16:03
that decides決定 how happy快樂 people are.
354
948000
3000
也不太重要
16:06
But now, because the reflective反光 self is in charge收費,
355
951000
4000
不過,既然現在是由思考主導
16:10
you may可能 end結束 up -- some people may可能 end結束 up
356
955000
2000
一些人可能會得到一個結論
16:12
moving移動 to California加州.
357
957000
2000
就是搬到加州
16:14
And it's sort分類 of interesting有趣 to trace跟踪 what is going to happen發生
358
959000
3000
追蹤為了更快樂而搬去加州的人
16:17
to people who move移動 to California加州 in the hope希望 of getting得到 happier幸福.
359
962000
3000
觀看他們的後續發展,相當有趣
16:20
Well, their experiencing經歷 self
360
965000
2000
他們的經驗的我
16:22
is not going to get happier幸福.
361
967000
2000
不會變得比較快樂
16:24
We know that.
362
969000
2000
這我們都知道
16:27
But one thing will happen發生: They will think they are happier幸福,
363
972000
3000
不過一件事發生了,他們覺得自己快樂多了.
16:30
because, when they think about it,
364
975000
4000
因為在他們思考時
16:34
they'll他們會 be reminded提醒 of how horrible可怕 the weather天氣 was in Ohio俄亥俄州,
365
979000
4000
他們會想到俄亥俄州的壞天氣
16:38
and they will feel they made製作 the right decision決定.
366
983000
3000
並覺得自己做了正確的決定
16:41
It is very difficult
367
986000
2000
要理解快樂生活
16:43
to think straight直行 about well-being福利,
368
988000
2000
實在很難
16:45
and I hope希望 I have given特定 you a sense
369
990000
3000
我希望已經讓你們有點概念
16:48
of how difficult it is.
370
993000
2000
明白這到底有多難
16:50
Thank you.
371
995000
2000
謝謝
16:52
(Applause掌聲)
372
997000
3000
(掌聲)
16:55
Chris克里斯 Anderson安德森: Thank you. I've got a question for you.
373
1000000
3000
謝謝你的演說,我有問題想請教你
16:59
Thank you so much.
374
1004000
2000
真的很感謝你
17:01
Now, when we were on the phone電話 a few少數 weeks ago,
375
1006000
4000
我們幾星期前在電話裡
17:05
you mentioned提到 to me that there was quite相當 an interesting有趣 result結果
376
1010000
3000
你提過蓋洛普的調查中
17:08
came來了 out of that Gallup蓋洛普 survey調查.
377
1013000
2000
有個有趣的發現
17:10
Is that something you can share分享
378
1015000
2000
現在你可以跟我們分享一下嗎
17:12
since以來 you do have a few少數 moments瞬間 left now?
379
1017000
2000
還有幾分鐘
17:14
Daniel丹尼爾 Kahneman卡尼曼: Sure.
380
1019000
2000
好的
17:16
I think the most interesting有趣 result結果 that we found發現 in the Gallup蓋洛普 survey調查
381
1021000
3000
我想蓋洛普調查的發現,最有趣的
17:19
is a number, which哪一個 we absolutely絕對 did not expect期望 to find.
382
1024000
3000
是個數字,能發現它實屬意外
17:22
We found發現 that with respect尊重 to the happiness幸福
383
1027000
2000
我們發現關於經驗我的快樂
17:24
of the experiencing經歷 self.
384
1029000
3000
有個現象
17:27
When we looked看著 at how feelings情懷,
385
1032000
5000
就是人的感覺
17:32
vary變化 with income收入.
386
1037000
2000
會隨收入而不同
17:34
And it turns out that, below下面 an income收入
387
1039000
3000
結果發現,對美國人而言
17:37
of 60,000 dollars美元 a year, for Americans美國人 --
388
1042000
3000
年收入低於6萬美元的人
17:40
and that's a very large sample樣品 of Americans美國人, like 600,000,
389
1045000
3000
佔了樣本中很大一部份,約60萬
17:43
so it's a large representative代表 sample樣品 --
390
1048000
2000
但這些年收入低於60萬美元的樣本
17:45
below下面 an income收入 of 600,000 dollars美元 a year...
391
1050000
2000
相當具有指標性
17:47
CACA: 60,000.
392
1052000
2000
是6萬美元
17:49
DKDK: 60,000.
393
1054000
2000
6萬美元
17:51
(Laughter笑聲)
394
1056000
2000
(笑聲)
17:53
60,000 dollars美元 a year, people are unhappy不快樂,
395
1058000
4000
年收入6萬美元的人不快樂
17:57
and they get progressively逐步 unhappier不快樂 the poorer they get.
396
1062000
3000
而且越窮的人數據顯示越不快樂
18:00
Above以上 that, we get an absolutely絕對 flat平面 line.
397
1065000
3000
6萬以上,我們得到一條水平線
18:03
I mean I've rarely很少 seen看到 lines so flat平面.
398
1068000
3000
難得看到這麼平坦的線
18:06
Clearly明確地, what is happening事件 is
399
1071000
2000
顯然
18:08
money does not buy購買 you experiential體驗 happiness幸福,
400
1073000
3000
金錢買不了經驗的快樂
18:11
but lack缺乏 of money certainly當然 buys購買 you misery苦難,
401
1076000
3000
但沒錢除了悲慘什麼都買不到
18:14
and we can measure測量 that misery苦難
402
1079000
2000
而悲慘的程度差別
18:16
very, very clearly明確地.
403
1081000
2000
顯而易見
18:18
In terms條款 of the other self, the remembering記憶 self,
404
1083000
3000
就另一個我,記憶的我而言
18:21
you get a different不同 story故事.
405
1086000
2000
你有了不一樣的故事
18:23
The more money you earn, the more satisfied滿意 you are.
406
1088000
3000
你賺得越多就越滿意
18:26
That does not hold保持 for emotions情緒.
407
1091000
2000
跟情感沒有關係
18:28
CACA: But Danny丹尼, the whole整個 American美國 endeavor努力 is about
408
1093000
3000
可是丹尼,所有美國人奮鬥的目標是
18:31
life, liberty自由, the pursuit追求 of happiness幸福.
409
1096000
3000
生命,自由和追求幸福的權利
18:34
If people took seriously認真地 that finding發現,
410
1099000
4000
如果大家認真看待這項發現
18:38
I mean, it seems似乎 to turn upside上邊 down
411
1103000
3000
似乎就顛覆了
18:41
everything we believe about, like for example,
412
1106000
2000
我們相信的一切
18:43
taxation稅收 policy政策 and so forth向前.
413
1108000
2000
好比課稅政策等
18:45
Is there any chance機會 that politicians政治家, that the country國家 generally通常,
414
1110000
3000
政治人物,國家有沒有可能
18:48
would take a finding發現 like that seriously認真地
415
1113000
3000
正視這樣的發現
18:51
and run public上市 policy政策 based基於 on it?
416
1116000
2000
依此施政
18:53
DKDK: You know I think that there is recognition承認
417
1118000
2000
我想快樂研究在政策中
18:55
of the role角色 of happiness幸福 research研究 in public上市 policy政策.
418
1120000
3000
所扮演的角色這項認知
18:58
The recognition承認 is going to be slow in the United聯合的 States狀態,
419
1123000
2000
在美國散播速度緩慢
19:00
no question about that,
420
1125000
2000
無庸置疑
19:02
but in the U.K., it is happening事件,
421
1127000
2000
但在英國,他正持續發酵
19:04
and in other countries國家 it is happening事件.
422
1129000
2000
在其他國家也是
19:06
People are recognizing認識 that they ought應該
423
1131000
3000
一般人開始認知到
19:09
to be thinking思維 of happiness幸福
424
1134000
2000
他們應該在思考政策時
19:11
when they think of public上市 policy政策.
425
1136000
2000
同時將快樂納入考量
19:13
It's going to take a while,
426
1138000
2000
這要花些時間
19:15
and people are going to debate辯論
427
1140000
3000
人民將開始思考
19:18
whether是否 they want to study研究 experience經驗 happiness幸福,
428
1143000
2000
他們要的是經驗的快樂
19:20
or whether是否 they want to study研究 life evaluation評測,
429
1145000
2000
或是要為生活打分數
19:22
so we need to have that debate辯論 fairly相當 soon不久.
430
1147000
3000
因此很快,我們將要理解這個問題
19:25
How to enhance提高 happiness幸福
431
1150000
2000
如何增進快樂
19:27
goes very different不同 ways方法 depending根據 on how you think,
432
1152000
3000
有各種不同方式,事關你怎麼想
19:30
and whether是否 you think of the remembering記憶 self
433
1155000
2000
想的是記憶的我
19:32
or you think of the experiencing經歷 self.
434
1157000
2000
還是經驗的我
19:34
This is going to influence影響 policy政策, I think, in years年份 to come.
435
1159000
3000
我想幾年之內,這將影響政策
19:37
In the United聯合的 States狀態, efforts努力 are being存在 made製作
436
1162000
3000
美國內付出許多努力
19:40
to measure測量 the experience經驗 happiness幸福 of the population人口.
437
1165000
3000
來衡量大眾經驗的快樂
19:43
This is going to be, I think, within the next下一個 decade or two,
438
1168000
3000
我想,十年到二十年內
19:46
part部分 of national國民 statistics統計.
439
1171000
2000
這將列為國內統計數據的一部份
19:48
CACA: Well, it seems似乎 to me that this issue問題 will -- or at least最小 should be --
440
1173000
4000
這個議題對我來說似乎
19:52
the most interesting有趣 policy政策 discussion討論 to track跟踪
441
1177000
2000
至少是幾年內
19:54
over the next下一個 few少數 years年份.
442
1179000
2000
最有趣的政策討論議題
19:56
Thank you so much for inventing發明了 behavioral行為的 economics經濟學.
443
1181000
2000
感謝你創造了行為經濟學
19:58
Thank you, Danny丹尼 Kahneman卡尼曼.
444
1183000
2000
感謝丹尼爾卡內曼
Translated by SHU-AN WU
Reviewed by Adrienne Lin

▲Back to top

ABOUT THE SPEAKER
Daniel Kahneman - Behavioral economics founder
Widely regarded as the world's most influential living psychologist, Daniel Kahneman won the Nobel in Economics for his pioneering work in behavioral economics -- exploring the irrational ways we make decisions about risk.

Why you should listen

Daniel Kahneman is an eminence grise for the Freakonomics crowd. In the mid-1970s, with his collaborator Amos Tversky, he was among the first academics to pick apart exactly why we make "wrong" decisions. In their 1979 paper on prospect theory, Kahneman and Tversky examined a simple problem of economic risk. And rather than stating the optimal, rational answer, as an economist of the time might have, they quantified how most real people, consistently, make a less-rational choice. Their work treated economics not as a perfect or self-correcting machine, but as a system prey to quirks of human perception. The field of behavioral economics was born.

Kahneman was awarded the Nobel Memorial prize in 2002 for his work with Tversky, who died before the award was bestowed. In a lovely passage in his Nobel biography, Kahneman looks back on his deep collaboration with Tversky and calls for a new form of academic cooperation, marked not by turf battles but by "adversarial collaboration," a good-faith effort by unlike minds to conduct joint research, critiquing each other in the service of an ideal of truth to which both can contribute.

More profile about the speaker
Daniel Kahneman | Speaker | TED.com

Data provided by TED.

This site was created in May 2015 and the last update was on January 12, 2020. It will no longer be updated.

We are currently creating a new site called "eng.lish.video" and would be grateful if you could access it.

If you have any questions or suggestions, please feel free to write comments in your language on the contact form.

Privacy Policy

Developer's Blog

Buy Me A Coffee